School Psychology International

School Psychology International

sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav

DOI: 10.1177/0143034312446892

spi.sagepub.com

Article

Cultural models of education and academic performance for Native American and European American students

Stephanie A. Fryberg and Rebecca Covarrubias University of Arizona, USA

Jacob A. Burack McGill University, Canada

Abstract

We examined the role of cultural representations of self (i.e., interdependence and

independence) and positive relationships (i.e., trust for teachers) in academic perform-

ance (i.e., self-reported grades) for Native American (N¼ 41) and European American (N¼ 49) high school students. The Native American students endorsed marginally more interdependent representation of self and marginally less trust for teachers

than did the European American students. While interdependent representations of

self and trust for teachers were positively related for the Native American students,

neither cultural representations of self were related to trust for teachers for the

European American students. However, with respect to academic performance, inter-

dependent representations of self and trust for teachers were positively related to

academic performance for the Native American students. Conversely, independent

and interdependent representations of self were positively related to academic per-

formance for the European American students, but trust for teachers was not asso-

ciated with academic performance. Finally, as predicted, culturally congruent

representations of self predicted academic performance. Specifically, trust for teachers

and interdependent representations of self positively predicted academic performance

for Native American students, whereas only independent representations of self pre-

dicted academic performance for European American students. Implications for cultur-

ally congruent models of education are discussed.

Corresponding author:

Stephanie A. Fryberg, Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, 1503 E. University Boulevard, Tucson,

AZ 85721, USA.

Email: fryberg@u.arizona.edu

Keywords

Academic performance, culture, Native American, social identity

In U.S. public schools, possessing a social identity that is commonly underrepre- sented in the classroom (e.g., racial-ethnic identity, working-class identity) is a strong indicator of disparities in academic achievement. For example, in the 12 states with the largest populations of Native American students, graduation rates reveal that while 71.4% of all students graduate from high school, fewer than 50% of Native American students graduate (Faircloth & Tippeconic, 2010). Efforts to close the achievement gap range from eliminating negative stereotypes and low expectancies of underrepresented racial-ethnic minority students to improving tea- chers’ understandings of cultural differences and culture- and self-relevant curricu- lum for racial-ethnic minority students. These approaches contend that when the school environment is free from negative stereotypes (Davies, Spencer, & Steele, 2005; Markus, Steele & Steele, 2000; Purdie-Vaughns, Steele, Davies, Ditlmann, & Crosby, 2008) and the curriculum is perceived as self-relevant (Gay, 2004; Ladson- Billings, 1995; Maehr, 2008), students will experience identity safety—they will per- ceive the school environment as a place where they belong and can be successful.

Research reveals, however, that even subtle cues conveyed in the school envir- onment can affect feelings of identity safety and self-relevance, and ultimately foster or undermine academic performance (Cohen, Garcia, Apfel, & Master, 2006; Stephens, Fryberg, Markus, Johnson, & Covarrubias, 2012; Walton & Cohen, 2007). For instance, Cohen and colleagues (2006) found that a small, subtle intervention aimed at affirming students’ sense of self and undermining negative stereotypes in school, improved grades and alleviated the achievement gap by 40%. These small interventions, however, are not limited to reversing the negative effects of stereotypes. Stephens and colleagues (2012) found, for instance, that small interventions also alleviate the effects of cultural incongruities. Specifically, over the first two years of college, they found that when the cultural norms (i.e., encouraging independence, choice, and self-expression) match, as they did for continuing-generation college students whose parents have four-year college degrees, the students perform better (i.e., higher grades) than when they do not match, as was the case for first-generation college students whose parents do not have four-year college degrees. To reduce this disparity in performance, the researchers provided a culture-relevant message that subtly framed the university context as more interdependent. Much like the intervention by Cohen and col- leagues, this small change in framing alleviated the discrepancies in performance between first-generation and continuing-generation college students.

In this study, we provide an initial test of how subtle messages about the good or right way to be a person (i.e., cultural representations of self) influences academic achievement for both Native American and European American students. Research reveals that Native American and European American students endorse

440 School Psychology International 34(4)

different models of self and that these models have implications for educational experience (Boykin et al., 2005; Fryberg & Markus, 2007; Li 2003, 2005; Stephens et al., 2012). We anticipate that when students endorse culturally congruent rep- resentations of self—Native American students will endorse an interdependent representation of self and European American students will endorse an independ- ent representation of self—they will also report better academic performance.

Cultural models of education

When students enter the classroom, they bring with them frameworks of tacit assumptions and meanings that reflect their social and developmental experiences (Resnick, 1994). Cognitive anthropologists refer to these assumptions and mean- ings as ‘cultural models’ (D’Andrade, 1981, 1995; Holland & Quinn, 1987; Shore, 1996; Sperber, 1985). Building on this work, Fryberg and Markus (2007, p. 237) used the term ‘cultural models of education’ to refer to the patterns of ideas and practices that are implicit in education contexts, including ideas about who is a good student, the purpose of getting an education, and the nature of the teacher- student relationship. The authors argue that while mainstream educational con- texts may appear to promote a ‘neutral‘ model of education, they actually foster a culture-specific model of education. For example, in university contexts, which typically represent middle-class European American values, the good or appropri- ate way to be a self is to be independent and separate from others and from the social context. This view has been termed the independent representation of self (Markus & Kitayama, 2010, Markus, Mullally, & Kitayama, 1997). Other cultural contexts, such as those in East Asian, Latino, Native American, and African con- texts, foster a view of the self as fundamentally connected to others and to the surrounding social context. This view has been termed interdependent representa- tions of the self.

In classrooms that reflect and foster independent representations of self, ‘learning’ is largely about the development of autonomous and independent ways of thinking (Bellah et al., 1985; Bruner, 1996; Greenfield, Trumbull, & Keller, 2006; Greenfield, Trumbull, & Rothstein-Fisch, 2003; Tharp, 1994). Learning styles are motivated by widely held cultural values of individuality, uniqueness, and the importance of making individual choices (Tobin, Wu, & Davidson, 1989). Good students should take individual responsibility for their academic success and should strive to distinguish themselves from others (Plaut & Markus, 2005). Consequently, individual competition and achievement are gen- erally more valued than collaboration and cooperation (Green, 2006; Platow & Shave, 1995; Trumbull, Rothstein-Fisch, & Greenfield, 2000). Teachers reify these values by focusing on and rewarding individual effort and achievement, and eval- uating individual contributions to group performance (Deyhle, 1985, 1986, 1992; Fiske, Kitayama, Markus, & Nisbett, 1998).

In classrooms that foster interdependent representations of self, however, learn- ing occurs primarily in interactions with others (Boykin et al., 2005; Escalante &

Fryberg et al. 441

Dirmann, 1990; Greenfield et al., 2003; Tobin et al., 1989; Trumbull et al., 2000). In these contexts, perceived social support, mentorship, maintaining strong connec- tions to the community, and trusting relationships with teachers positively impact persistence and academic achievement (Cole, Matheson, & Anisman, 2007; Cummins, 1992; Deyhle & Margonis, 1995; Fryberg & Markus, 2007; Gloria & Kurpius, 2001). For example, Native American students who engaged in a collab- orative learning environment demonstrated improved academic learning and motivation (Deyhle & Swisher, 1997).

One reason that collaborative learning environments are effective in creating positive academic outcomes is because they allow Native American students to develop positive, trusting relationships in school. Fryberg and Markus (2007), for instance, found that interdependent representations of self and trust for tea- chers were positively associated for Native American college students, but not for European American college students. The study suggested that if Native American students are more relational or interdependent in their representations of self, then trust is likely to be an essential feature of the student-teacher relationship. One limitation, however, is that while the study shows that Native American and European American college students endorse different representations of self and different expectations for the student-teacher relationship, it does not examine whether these factors are important for academic achievement and for younger students (i.e., high school students), a time in schooling where students experience greater discrepancies in achievement.

In this initial study, we assess the role of Native American and European American high school students’ cultural representations of self (i.e., interdependence versus independence) and positive relationships (i.e., trust for teachers) in academic performance (i.e., self-reported grades). Based on prior research, we anticipate that Native American students will report more inter- dependent representations of self and less trust for teachers than European American students, and that the two groups will not differ in terms of independent representations of self. In terms of academic performance, we anticipate that inter- dependent representations of self and trust for teachers (i.e., an indication of posi- tive relationships in school) will predict grades for Native American students, whereas independent representations of self will predict grades for European American students.

Method

Participants

The participants included 41 Native American (24 females; M age¼ 15.63, SD¼ 1.53) and 49 European American (26 females; M age¼ 15.45, SD¼ 1.21) high school students from a rural location in Washington State, USA. Among the approximately 1600 students enrolled in the school, 9% are Native

442 School Psychology International 34(4)

American. The students were informed of the purpose and logistics of the study in their English classes and were randomly selected from the consent forms signed by both the student and parents or guardians.

Procedure and materials

All the students were instructed that the purpose of the study was to learn more about their educational experiences. They were informed that their answers were completely confidential and that they could discontinue the study at any time without penalty. At this time, students completed questionnaire packets that included the dependent measures.

Interdependence and independence were measured with four sub-scales (alphas ranging from 0.71 to 0.85) from Kato and Markus (1994). The Interdependence scale included 16 items (e.g., ‘It is important to me that I make a favorable impres- sion on others’, and ‘If someone helps me, I feel a strong obligation to return the favor sometime later’) that measured concern for others and self-other bond. The Independence scale consisted of 15 items (e.g., ‘I am unique—different from others in many respects’, and ‘No matter what is the situation or setting, I am always true to myself’) that measured self-other differentiation and self-knowledge. Students rated how well the items described them on a scale from 0 (doesn’t describe me at all) to 9 (describes me very much). The internal reliabilities for interdependence was a¼ 0.81 (Native American¼ 0.76; European American¼ 0.82) and independence was a¼ 0.78 (Native American¼ 0.68; European American¼ 0.75).

Trust for teachers was measured using a sub-set of five items from the Cultural Mistrust Inventory (Terrell & Terrell, 1981). Example items included: ‘Teachers often ask questions so they can get something on you’, ‘You usually get the grade you deserve from a teacher’, ‘You can talk to a teacher about things without worrying that it will come back on you later’, and ‘Teachers often twist things to make students look bad’. Students were asked to rate how much they disagreed or agreed with each statement on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). All the questions were recoded such that a higher score indicated higher trust. The internal reliability for the measure was psychometrically sufficient, a¼ 0.74 (Native American¼ 0.74; European American¼ 0.79).

Finally, to measure academic performance, students were asked to rate on an eight-point scale, with 0 indicating mostly F’s and 8 indicating mostly A’s, the following item: ‘What grades do you usually get in school?’

Results

Independent/interdependent representations of self

An ethnicity (Native American, European American) x gender (female, male) multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) for independent and interdependent

Fryberg et al. 443

representations of self was performed using age as a covariate (ns). No main effects for independent representations of self by ethnicity, F (1,85)¼ 0.02, p¼ 0.98, or gender, F (1,85)¼ 0.19, p¼ 0.92, were found, but main effects for interdependent representations of self by ethnicity, F (1, 85)¼ 3.19, p< 0.08, and by gender, F (1, 85)¼ 4.24, p< 0.05, were found. Specifically, Native American students were marginally more interdependent than European American students, and female students were more interdependent than male students. See Figure 1 for means.

Trust for teachers

An ethnicity (Native American, European American) � gender analysis of variance (ANOVA) on trust for teachers revealed a marginal main effect for ethnicity, F (1, 61)¼ 2.89, p< 0.09. Native American students were somewhat less trusting of teachers (M¼ 3.30, SD¼ 1.19) than were European American students (M¼ 3.69, SD¼ 1.38).

Relationship between representations of self and trust for teachers

For Native American high school students, a positive relationship was found between interdependent representations of self and both trust for teachers (r¼ 0.36, p< 0.05) and self-reported grades (r¼ 0.50, p< 0.001). Trust for teachers was also positively correlated with self-reported grades (r¼ 0.44, p< 0.01). However, no relationship was found between independent representations of self and trust for teachers (r¼�0.04, p¼ 0.90) or academic performance (r¼ 0.16, ns).

4

5

6

7

8

Independence Interdependence Independence Interdependence

American Indian European American

M ea

n Sc

or e

Female Male

Figure 1. Mean scores of independent and interdependent representations of self by ethnicity

and gender.

444 School Psychology International 34(4)

For European American high school students, significant relationships were found between both independent representations of self and academic performance (r¼ 0.32, p< 0.05) and interdependent representations of self and academic per- formance (r¼ 0.31, p< 0.05). However, no significant relationships were found between interdependent representations of self and trust for teachers (r¼ 0.18, p¼ 0.50) independent representations of self and trust for teachers (r¼ 0.03, p¼ 0.92), or between trust for teacher and grades (r¼ 0.13, p¼ 0.61).

Academic performance

Predicting self-reported academic performance. An ethnicity (Native American, European American) x gender ANOVA was used to compare self-reported grades. Main effects were found for both ethnicity and gender, as the European American students (M¼ 6.06, SD¼ 1.53) reported higher grades than the Native American students (M¼ 5.10, SD¼ 2.11), F (1, 87)¼ 6.15, p< 0.02, and the female students reported higher grades (M¼ 6.04, SD¼ 1.78) than the male students (M¼ 5.05, SD¼ 1.96), F (1, 87)¼ 6.33, p< 0.02.

We first examined how cultural representations of self (independent and inter- dependent), trust for teachers, and ethnicities were related to grades. We reasoned that if an interaction was found between ethnicity and the other predictor variables, then performing separate analyses for each ethnic group would be an appropriate next step. A stepwise regression with grades as the dependent variable and with ethnicity, independent representations of self, interdependent representations of self, independent representations of self by ethnicity interaction, and interdepend- ent representations of self by ethnicity interaction as the independent variables revealed that interdependent representations of self by ethnicity interaction, p< 0.001, was the first significant predictor of grades, and independent represen- tations of self, p< .05, was the second significant predictor variable.

In the second set of analyses, separate stepwise regressions were performed for independent and interdependent representations of self. In the first regression, grades were the dependent variable while ethnicity, independent representations of self, and independent representations of self by ethnicity were the independent variables. The only significant finding was the independent representations of self by cultural context interaction, p< 0.001. Similarly, in the second regression, grades were the dependent variable and ethnicity, interdependent representations of self, and interdependent representations of self by ethnicity were the independent variables. Again, the only significant variable was the interdependent representa- tions of self by ethnicity interaction, p< 0.001.

Based on these analyses, we concluded that ethnicity interacts differently with the predictor variables and, thus, regression analyses were run separately for both ethnic groups. In both stepwise regressions, grades were the dependent variable, and trust for teachers, independent representations of self, and interdependent representations of self were the independent variables (see Table 1 for standardized regression coefficients). For the Native American students, the two predictor

Fryberg et al. 445

variables of trust for teachers, p< 0.01, and interdependent representations of self, p< 0.01, were significant. For the European American students, only the predictor variable of independent representations of self was significant, p< 0.01.

Discussion

The purpose of the present study was to provide preliminary evidence for how cultural representations of self (interdependent self versus independent self) and relationships in school (i.e., trust for teachers) contribute to academic performance for Native American and European American high school students. We anticipated that if students reported a culturally congruent representation of self—Native American students endorse an interdependent self and European American stu- dents endorse an independent self—then they would also report higher academic performance (i.e., grades).

We found that Native American students marginally endorsed more inter- dependent representation of self and less trust for teachers than European American students. Consistent with Fryberg and Markus (2007), interdependent representations of self and trust for teachers were positively related for Native American students. Neither independent nor interdependent representations of self were related to trust for teachers for European American students. With respect to academic performance, interdependent representations of self and trust for teachers were positively related to academic performance for Native American students. Conversely, independent and interdependent representations of self were positively related to academic performance for European American students, but trust for teachers was not associated with representations of self or academic per- formance. In terms of predicting academic performance (i.e., self-reported grades), as expected, trust for teachers and interdependent representations of self positively predictedperformance for Native American students, whereas only independent representations of self predicted performance for European American students.

The theoretical and practical implications of this study suggest that the largely independent focus of mainstream educational contexts (Fryberg & Markus, 2007; Li, 2003; Stephens et al, 2011) may contribute to the underperformance of Native American students, as in the case of traditional classrooms in which ‘learning’ is construed as being about the development of autonomous thinking (Bellah et al.,

Table 1. Standardized regression coefficients for predictors of self-reported grades

Predictor variables American Indians European Americans

Independent representations of self �0.28y 0.39* Interdependent representations of self 0.38** 0.25

Trust for teachers 0.41** 0.26

R2 0.36*** 0.15*

Note: yp< 0.10; *p< 0.05; **p< .01; ***p< 0.001.

446 School Psychology International 34(4)

1985; Bruner, 1996; Tharp, 1994). Given that this norm is culturally incongruent for Native American students, the focus on autonomous thinking or independence may inadvertently convey to Native American students that they do not belong and cannot be successful in that environment. Accordingly, building on the work of social identity threat (Davies et al., 2005; Purdie-Vaughns et al., 2008; Walton & Cohen, 2007), the independent focus contributes to the perception that traditional classrooms are not identity safe contexts.

The findings from this study also suggest a need to rethink the nature of the student-teacher relationship. Most American teachers do not assume that success- fully teaching students requires building positive, trusting relationships; but the findings suggest that this expectation is critical for potentially alleviating the achievement gap for Native American students. The need for positive trusting relationships and interdependent representations of self also highlights a gender difference as Native American female students as compared to male students reported higher interdependent representations of self, a strong predictor of per- formance in this group. This theoretical link may help explain some of the gender discrepancies between Native American females and males, but needs to be fol- lowed up in larger cohorts.

The findings are also relevant to theorizing about cultural models of educa- tion. Consistent with previous research on cultural models of education, the findings suggest that, much like first-generation college students (Stephens et al., 2011), Native American students may feel a greater sense of belonging and motivation when the education context matches their own cultural under- standings of self. Despite the lower than (national) average graduation rates among Native American high school students (Faircloth & Tippeconic, 2010), the findings from this study suggest that if teachers, school psychologists and administrators focus on building relationships and including interdependent ways of being in the classroom, they can create identity safe contexts and thus, enhance academic performance for Native American students. Further, it is recommended that school psychologists be directly involved in conducting these types of studies, implementing interventions based on these findings, and aiding other staff as they provide educational and psychological services for these types of indigenous populations. These types of findings have direct impli- cations, then, for the school psychologists who may provide the liaison assist- ance for teachers, administrators, and other school staff. School psychologists are in a unique position to inform school staff who work with indigenous students living in the U.S. and in other geographic locations and to help to implement best practice according to the findings of this study. Although future studies are needed to parse out the different approaches to creating culturally congruent models of education, the findings here suggest that teachers and administrators, with the help of sensitive and knowledgeable school psycholo- gists, can begin to alleviate the achievement gap by fostering relationships with Native American students and orienting classroom styles to meet these students own cultural understandings of self.

Fryberg et al. 447

Finally, issues experienced by Native American students are not dissimilar to those faced by Bedouins (Landsman, 1990) and Ethiopian immigrants (Rosenblum, Goldblatt, & Moin, 2008) in Israel. Furthermore, there are simila- rities to the experiences faced by other indigenous populations: the Maori in New Zealand (Little, Akin-Little, & Johansen, 2013); the rural Javanese villagers in Indonesia specifically post natural disaster (Seyle, Widyatmoko, & Cohen Silver, 2013); the Kuku in Southern Sudan (Laguarda & Woodward, 2013); the Indigenous Australian youth (Yeung, Craven, & Ali, 2013); and the Naskapi youth of Canada (Burack et al., 2013). Thus, all school personnel, particularly school/educational psychologists, are encouraged to attend to the findings of this special edition of School Psychology International, specifically if they work with rural and/or indigenous populations (see Yeung et al. for recommendations).

References

Bellah, R. N., Madsen, R., Sullivan, W.M., Swidler, A., & Tipton, S. M. (1985).Habits of the heart: Individualism and commitment in American life. New York, NY: Harper and Row.

Boykin, A. W., Albury, A., Tyler, K., Hurley, E., Bailey, C. T., & Miller, O. A. (2005). Culture-based perceptions of academic achievement among low-income elementary

students. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 11, 339–350, doi: 10.1037/ 1099-9809.11.4.339.

Bruner, J. (1996). The culture of education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Burack, J. A., D’Arrisso, A., Ponizovsky, V., Troop-Gordon, W., Mandour, T., Tootootsis, C., Robinson, S., Iarocci, G., & Fryberg, S. A. (2013). Friends and grades: Peer prefer- ence and attachment predict academic success among Naskapi youth. School Psychology

International, 34(4), 371–386. Cohen, G. L., Garcia, J., Apfel, N., & Master, A. (2006). Reducing the racial achievement

gap: A social-psychological intervention. Science, 313, 1307–1310, doi: 10.1126/

science.1128317. Cole, B., Matheson, K., & Anisman, H. (2007). The moderating role of ethnic identity and

social support on relations between well-being and academic performance. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 37, 592–615, doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2007.00176.x.

Cummins, J. (1992). The empowerment of Indian students. In J. Reyhner (Ed.), Teaching American Indian students (pp. 3–12). Norman, OK: University of Oklahoma Press.

D’Andrade, R. (1981). The cultural part of cognition. Cognitive Science, 5, 179–195, doi:

10.1207/s15516709cog0503_1. D’Andrade, R. (1995). The development of cognitive anthropology. Cambridge, MA:

Cambridge University Press.

Davies, P., Spencer, S., & Steele, C. (2005). Clearing the air: Identity safety moderates the effects of stereotype threat on women’s leadership aspirations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 276–287, doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.2.276.

Deyhle, D. (1985). Testing among Navajo and Anglo students: Another consideration of

cultural bias. Journal of Educational Equity and Leadership, 5, 119–131. Deyhle, D. (1986). Success and failure: A micro-ethnographic comparison of Navajo and

Anglo students’ perceptions of testing. Curriculum Inquiry, 16, 365–389, doi: 10.2307/

1179428.

448 School Psychology International 34(4)

Deyhle, D. (1992). Constructing failure and maintaining cultural identity: Navajo and Ute

school leavers. Journal of American Indian Education, 31, 24–47. Deyhle, D., & Margonis, F. (1995). Navajo mothers and daughters: Schools, jobs,

and the family. Anthropology and Education Quarterly, 26, 135–167, doi: 10.1525/

aeq.1995.26.2.05x1252d. Deyhle, D., & Swisher, K. (1997). Research in American Indian and Alaska Native educa-

tion: From assimilation to self-determination. Review of Research in Education, 22, 113–194, doi: 10.3102/0091732X022001113.

Escalante, J., & Dirmann, J. (1990). The Jaime Escalante math program. Journal of Negro Education, 59, 407–423, doi: 10.2307/2295573.

Faircloth, S. C. & Tippeconic, J. W., III. (2010). The dropout/graduation crisis among

American Indian and Alaska Native students: Failure to respond places the future of Native peoples at risk. Published report in collaboration with the Civil Rights Project at UCLA and the Center for the Study of Leadership in American Indian

Education at Pennsylvania State University. Retrieved from http://civilrightsproject. ucla.edu/research/k-12-education/school-dropouts/the-dropout-graduation-crisis-among- american-indian-and-alaska-native-students-failure-to-respond-places-the-future-of-native- peoples-at-risk/faircloth-tippeconnic-native-american-dropouts.pdf.

Fiske, A. P., Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., & Nisbett, R. (1998). The cultural matrix of social psychology. In D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 915–981). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Fryberg, S. A., & Markus, H. R. (2007). Cultural models of education in American Indian, Asian American and European American contexts. Social Psychology of Education, 10, 213–246, doi: 10.1007/s11218-007-9017-z.

Gay, G. (2004). Beyond Brown: Promoting equality through multicultural education. Journal of Curriculum and Supervision, 19, 193–216, doi: 10.1207/s15327892mcp0604_5.

Gloria, A. M., & Kurpius, S. E. R. (2001). Influences of self-beliefs, social support, and

comfort in the university environment on the academic nonpersistence decisions of American Indian undergraduates. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 7, 88–102, doi: 10.1207/s15327892mcp0604_5.

Green, E. (2006). Successful or friendly? Inferring achievement and relational competence

from individualist and collectivist attitudes. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 65, 25–36, doi: 10.1024/1421-0185.65.1.25.

Greenfield, P. M., Trumbull, E., & Keller, H. (2006). Cultural conceptions of learning and

development. In P. A. Alexander, & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psych- ology (pp. 675–692). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Greenfield, P. M., Trumbull, E. T., & Rothstein-Fisch, C. (2003). Bridging cultures. Cross-

Cultural Psychology Bulletin, 37, 6–16, doi: 10.1207/S1532771XJLE0203_01. Holland, D., & Quinn, N. (Eds.) (1987). Cultural models in language and thought. New York,

NY: Cambridge University Press.

Kato, K., & Markus, H. R. (1994). Independence-Interdependence Scale. In E. Hatfield, J. T. Cacioppo, & R. L. Rapson (Eds.), Emotional contagion (p. 153). Paris: Cambridge University Press.

Ladson-Billings, G. (1995). But that’s just good teaching! The case for culturally relevant

pedagogy. Theory into Practice, 34, 159–165, doi: 10.1080/00405849509543675. Laguarda, A. I., & Woodward, W. P. (2013). They own this: Mother tongue instruction for

indigenous Kuku children of Southern Sudan. School Psychology International, 34(4),

453–469.

Fryberg et al. 449

Landesman, M. S. (1990). Psychological services for Bedouin schools in southern Israel: A

chronicle and critique. School Psychology International, 11, 125–132, doi: 10.1177/ 0143034390112006.

Li, J. (2003). US and Chinese cultural beliefs about learning. Journal of Educational

Psychology, 95, 258–267, doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.95.2.258. Li, J. (2005). Mind or virtue: Western and Chinese beliefs about learning. Current Directions

in Psychological Science, 14, 190–194, doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00362.x. Little, S. G., Akin-Little, K. A., & Johansen, A. (2013). Bi-cultural Aotearoa/New Zealand:

Provision of psychological services to the Maori population of rural New Zealand: Combining best practice with cultural considerations. School Psychology International, 34(4), 428–438.

Maehr, M. (2008). Culture and achievement motivation. International Journal of Psychology, 43, 917–918, doi: 10.1080/00207590701838162.

Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (2010). Cultures and selves: A cycle of mutual constitution.

Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 420–430, doi: 10.1177/1745691610375557. Markus, H. R., Mullally, P., & Kitayama, S. (1997). Selfways: Diversity in modes of cultural

participation. In U. Neisser, & D. Jopling (Eds.), The conceptual self in context: Culture, experience, self-understanding (pp. 13–61). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Markus, H. R., Steele, C. M., & Steele, D. M. (2000). Colorblindness as a barrier to inclu- sion: Assimilation and nonimmigrant minorities. Daedalus, 129, 233–259.

Platow, M., & Shave, R. (1995). Social value orientations and the expression of achievement

motivation. Journal of Social Psychology, 135, 71–81, doi: 10.1080/ 00224545.1995.9711404.

Plaut, V. C., & Markus, H. R. (2005). The ‘inside’ story: A cultural-historical analysis of

being smart and motivated, American Style. In C. Dweck, & A. Elliott (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 457–487). New York, NY: Guilford.

Purdie-Vaughns, V., Steele, C. M., Davies, P. G., Ditlmann, R., & Crosby, J. R. (2008).

Social identity contingencies: How diversity cues signal threat or safety for African Americans in mainstream institutions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 615–630, doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.4.615.

Resnick, L. B. (1994). Situated rationalism: Biological and social preparation for learning.

In Hirschfeld, & Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 474–493). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Rosenblum, S., Goldblatt, H., & Moin, V. (2008). The hidden dropout phenomenon

among immigrant high-school students: The case of Ethiopian adolescents in Israel–A pilot study. School Psychology International, 29, 105–127, doi: 10.1177/ 0143034307088506.

Seyle, D. C., Widyatmoko, C. S., & Cohen Silver, R. (2013). Coping with natural disasters in Yogyakarta, Indonesia: A study of elementary school teacher. School Psychology International, 34(4), 387–404.

Shore, B. (1996). Culture in mind: Cognition, culture, and the problem of meaning. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Sperber, D. (1985). Anthropology and psychology: Towards an epidemiology of represen- tations. Man, 20, 73–89, doi: 10.2307/2802222.

Stephens, N. M., Fryberg, S. A., Markus, H. R., Johnson, C. & Covarrubias, R. (2012, March 5). Unseen disadvantage: How American universities’ focus on independence undermines the academic performance of first-generation college students. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1037/a0027143.

450 School Psychology International 34(4)

Tharp, R. (1994). Intergroup differences among Native Americans in socialization and

child cognition: An ethnogenetic analysis. In P. Greenfield, & R. Cocking (Eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minority child development (pp. 87–105). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Terrell, F., & Terrell, S. (1981). An inventory to measure cultural mistrust among Blacks. Western Journal of Black Studies, 5, 180–184.

Tobin, J. J., Wu, D. Y., & Davidson, D. H. (1989). Preschool in three cultures: Japan, China, and the United States. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Trumbull, E., Rothstein-Fisch, C., & Greenfield, P.M. (2000). Bridging cultures in our schools: New approaches that work. Retrieved from http://www.wested.org/online_ pubs/bridging/welcome.shtml.

Walton, G. M., & Cohen, G. L. (2007). A question of belonging: Race, social fit, and achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 82–96, doi: 10.1037/ 0022-3514.92.1.82.

Yeung, A. S., Craven, R. G., & Ali, J. (2013). Self concepts and educational outcomes of indigenous Australian students in urban and rural school settings. School Psychology International, 34(4), 405–427.

Stephanie A. Fryberg is an Associate Professor in the Department of Psychology and an Affiliate Faculty Member in American Indian Studies at the University of Arizona. She is currently on leave from the university, serving as the Director of Cultural Competency and Learning Improvement for the Marysville School District in Washington State. Her research examines how social identities, such as race, ethnicity, and social class, shape how people understand their own behavior, the behaviors of others, and the social worlds in which they participate. Stephanie received the Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues Louise Kidder Early Career Award, the University of Arizona Five Star Faculty Award, and was recently inducted into the Stanford University Multicultural Alumni Hall of Fame. Address: Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, 1503 E. University Boulevard, Tucson, AZ 85721, USA. Email: fryberg@u.arizona.edu

Rebecca Covarrubias recently earned her doctorate in Social Psychology at the University of Arizona. Using cultural and social psychological theories, her research examines the educational discrepancies and retention strategies for ethnic minority and working-class students. Rebecca has received the National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship, the Ford Foundation Predoctoral Diversity Fellowship, and the University of Arizona Graduate College Diversity Fellowship. Address: Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, 1503 E. University Boulevard, Tucson, AZ 85721, USA.

Jacob A. Burack, Ph.D., is Professor of School/Applied Child Psychology and Human Development in the Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology at McGill University, and Director of the McGill Youth Study

Fryberg et al. 451

Team (MYST). Jake, his students, and colleagues study predictors of academic success, social adaptation, and emotional well-being among adolescent students who live in First Nations communities. All their work is carried out within the context of the MYST logo of ‘‘a commitment to excellence in the study and education of all children’’.

452 School Psychology International 34(4)


Comments are closed.