Shared Authentic Leadership in Research Teams: Testing a Multiple Mediation Model

Shared Authentic Leadership in Research Teams: Testing a Multiple Mediation Model

https://doi.org/10.1177/1046496417732403

Small Group Research 1 –47

© The Author(s) 2017 Reprints and permissions:

sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/1046496417732403

journals.sagepub.com/home/sgr

Article

Shared Authentic Leadership in Research Teams: Testing a Multiple Mediation Model

Hannes Guenter1, William L. Gardner2, Kelly Davis McCauley3, Brandon Randolph-Seng4, and Veena P. Prabhu5

Abstract Research teams face complex leadership and coordination challenges. We propose shared authentic leadership (SAL) as a timely approach to addressing these challenges. Drawing from authentic and functional leadership theories, we posit a multiple mediation model that suggests three mechanisms whereby SAL influences team effectiveness: shared mental models (SMM), team trust, and team coordination. To test our hypotheses, we collected survey data on leadership and teamwork within 142 research teams that recently published an article in a peer-reviewed management journal. The results indicate team coordination represents the primary mediating mechanism accounting for the relationship between SAL and research team effectiveness. While teams with high trust and SMM felt more successful and were more satisfied, they were less successful in publishing in high-impact journals. We also found the four SAL dimensions (i.e., self-awareness, relational transparency, balanced processing, and internalized moral perspective) to associate differently with team effectiveness.

1Maastricht University, The Netherlands 2Texas Tech University, Lubbock, USA 3West Texas A&M University, Canyon, USA 4Texas A&M University–Commerce, USA 5California State University, Los Angeles, USA

Corresponding Author: Hannes Guenter, Department of Organization & Strategy, School of Business and Economics, Maastricht University, Tongersestraat 53, 6211 LM Maastricht, The Netherlands. Email: h.guenter@maastrichtuniversity.nl

732403 SGRXXX10.1177/1046496417732403Small Group ResearchGuenter et al. research-article2017

2 Small Group Research 00(0)

Keywords authentic leadership, shared leadership, team effectiveness, research team

Teams are increasingly asked to solve novel and complex problems, espe- cially in knowledge-intensive domains, such as in technology development, professional service work, or basic and applied research (Bruns, 2013; Kotha, George, & Srikanth, 2013). Yet, it is, “ever more difficult for any leader from above to have all of the knowledge, skills and abilities necessary to lead all aspects of knowledge work” (Pearce & Manz, 2005, p. 132), often creating a need for team members to share leadership responsibilities. As such, shared leadership emerges in a team to the extent that leader influence is distributed among team members (Carson, Tesluk, & Marrone, 2007). Of specific inter- est to the current research is shared authentic leadership (SAL; Hmieleski, Cole, & Baron, 2012), which—in broad terms—describes a mutual influence process whereby team members share leadership responsibilities in a fashion that reflects self-awareness, balanced processing of information, relational transparency toward one another, and an internalized moral perspective.

Authentic leadership describes a genuine form of leadership through which leaders remain true to their personal values and convictions, display consistency between their words and deeds, and thereby garner high levels of trust and performance from followers (Avolio & Gardner, 2005). Interestingly, the large majority of research has studied authentic leadership at the individual level (see Gardner, Cogliser, Davis, & Dickens, 2011). Hmieleski et al. (2012), however, argued that authentic leadership also takes place at the team level and that it can be shared. That is, leadership can be distributed among team members, change at varying points depend- ing on which member(s) meets the needs of the team at the time, and carried out in an authentic way that is respected by all members. This view aligns with a broader realization in management science that the notion of a sin- gular strong team leader may be outdated, especially since recent evidence suggests that leadership often is a shared team responsibility (see Wang, Waldman, & Zhang, 2014). Thus, we followed the lead of Hmieleski et al. (2012) and studied SAL and its effects at the team level. By defining authentic leadership at the team level we can extend authentic leadership theory to study questions that, while practically relevant, have hitherto received little research attention. Specifically, we assess the mediating mechanisms through which SAL influences team effectiveness. We posit that by examining how such teams authentically share leadership responsi- bilities, a replicable model for successfully sharing leadership in knowl- edge-intensive teams can be developed.

Guenter et al. 3

We make three major contributions to the literature. First, we unearth the mechanisms through which authentic leadership may exert influence on team effectiveness. Existing research is limited to singling out a particular mediator, such as positive affective tone (Hmieleski et al., 2012) or team- work behavior (Hannah, Walumbwa, & Fry, 2011). Instead, we develop a comprehensive, theoretically grounded framework that suggests three medi- ating mechanisms between SAL and team effectiveness (Luthans & Avolio, 2003; Zaccaro, Rittman, & Marks, 2001). We expect cognitive (i.e., shared mental models [SMM]), affective-motivational (i.e., team trust), and behav- ioral (i.e., team coordination) mechanisms to explain the SAL–effectiveness association. By developing and testing a multiple mediation model, we pro- vide a more complete answer as to why SAL may be related to team effec- tiveness, limit the risk of overestimating the importance of a single mediator, and develop the knowledge necessary to design interventions that target spe- cific mediating mechanisms (De Jong & Elfring, 2010; Wo, Ambrose, & Schminke, 2015).

Second, we study SAL at the component level. Although authentic lead- ership has been defined with regard to its four primary components (i.e., self-awareness, balanced processing, relational transparency, and internal- ized moral perspective; Gardner et al., 2011; Walumbwa, Avolio, Gardner, Wernsing, & Peterson, 2008), extant research on authentic leadership has rarely examined these separate components and their effects (Gardner et al., 2011). Neider and Schriesheim (2011), however, concluded from their development and assessment of the Authentic Leadership Inventory (ALI) that “future research might be best served by using the dimensions or scales separately rather than combining them all into a global measure” (p. 1156). Such separation allows for the identification of components that are most important in particular situations, as Gardner et al. (2011) and more recently, Banks, McCauley, Gardner, and Guler (2016), recommended. By investi- gating the effects of the SAL dimensions separately, we better align authen- tic leadership theory and measurement and hope to gain a more nuanced understanding of SAL.

Third, we focus on a specific kind of knowledge-intensive team, that is, research teams. A research team is a “group of researchers collaborating to produce scientific results, which are primarily communicated in the form of research articles” (Milojević, 2014, p. 3984). We study shared leadership in research teams because of the apparent shift in science from individual scholarship to scientific teamwork (Wuchty, Jones, & Uzzi, 2007), and because research teams often operate without a formally appointed leader, making them dependent on shared leadership (Jonsen et al., 2012). Although the sharing of leadership responsibilities is especially relevant in scientific

4 Small Group Research 00(0)

teams, it is also fraught with difficulties because scientific teams try to solve novel, complex problems while they are often geographically dis- persed and pressed for time (Jones, Wuchty, & Uzzi, 2008; Salazar, Lant, Fiore, & Salas, 2012). Unfortunately, our knowledge of how to master these collaborations and leadership challenges in research teams remains limited (see Salazar et al., 2012). In this study, we suggest that SAL can help address some of these challenges.

We focus on SAL because researchers are drawn to research teams by perceived opportunities for self-determination and autonomy—qualities cen- tral to authenticity and the self-authorship of one’s work and career choices (Kernis & Goldman, 2006). SAL is about being open in giving and receiving feedback and implies being skeptical toward one’s own viewpoints before making decisions. These qualities also facilitate scientific discoveries, and if widespread in a research team, should foster research productivity (e.g., Schilpzand, Herold, & Shalley, 2011). SAL, thus, may motivate research teams in ways that other leadership styles cannot.

Theoretical Background and Hypotheses

Authentic Leadership

Authentic leadership describes a form of leadership whereby the leader is “true to the self” (Gardner, Avolio, Luthans, May, & Walumbwa, 2005, p. 357). Authentic leaders act in accordance with their personal values and convictions, display consistency between their words and deeds, and thereby garner high levels of trust and performance from followers (Avolio & Gardner, 2005). Authentic leadership is related to the more general concept of authentic functioning, which arises from “the operation of one’s core or true self in one’s daily enterprise” (Kernis, 2003, p. 13). Authentic function- ing is manifest by being truthful, self-aware as to what is personally motivat- ing, and “staying true to personal values through behavior” (Leroy, Anseel, Gardner, & Sels, 2015, p. 1679). Unlike authentic functioning, though, authentic leadership goes beyond simple intra- and interpersonal authenticity, as it constitutes a form of influence whereby leadership and followership is realized (Gardner et al., 2005).

As noted in psychological conceptions of authenticity, it appears that to foster such authentic leader–follower relationships, authentic leaders pursue heightened levels of self-awareness, balanced processing, relational transpar- ency, and an internalized moral perspective for themselves and followers (see Kernis, 2003 and Kernis & Goldman, 2006 for more information on the theo- retical underpinnnings of authentic leadership).

Guenter et al. 5

Specifically, self-awareness reflects the extent to which one knows oneself; it provides a foundation for authenticity, in that one must first know the self to be true to that self. Balanced processing implies being open to process both positive and negative self-relevant information with minimal influence of ego- defensive mechanisms (Kernis, 2003). Relational transparency constitutes an ability and willingness to share personal information with close others, thereby making oneself vulnerable and fostering heightened levels of interpersonal trust (Gardner et al., 2005). An internalized moral perspective involves the internal regulation of moral reasoning and conduct, such that one makes ethi- cal decisions and engages in ethical behavior consistent with personal values (Walumbwa et al., 2008). By modeling these behaviors and promoting open and authentic relationships, authentic leaders strive to foster the authentic development of followers. The mechanisms whereby authentic leaders are posited to influence followers in teams include behavioral modeling, personal and organizational identification, emotional contagion, support for self-deter- mination, and social exchanges (Ilies, Morgeson, & Nahrgang, 2005).

Introducing SAL

Yammarino, Dionne, Schriesheim, and Dansereau (2008) argued that at the team level, “authentic leadership can be viewed as operating similarly to the shared leadership approach” (p. 701). Shared leadership describes an “emer- gent team property that results from the distribution of leadership influence across multiple team members” (Carson et al., 2007, p. 1218). To the extent that shared leadership is the norm in teams, leadership functions are distrib- uted among members (e.g., D’Innocenzo, Mathieu, & Kukenberger, 2016; Drescher, Korsgaard, Welpe, Picot, & Wigand, 2014), and members see it as their joint responsibility to lead each other toward goal achievement (Wang et al., 2014). Consistent with the shared leadership literature and in following Hmieleski et al. (2012), we define SAL as an emergent phenomenon charac- terized by mutual influence and distributed responsibility that helps teams develop their “positive psychological capabilities and promotes a positive climate consistent with members’ values and beliefs” (p. 1477).

Shared leadership behaviors differ from team processes. Wang et al. (2014) summarized that difference as follows:

The former [shared leadership] stresses distributed influence and responsibility among team members (Carson et al., 2007), while the latter [teamwork or team processes] pertains to a set of cooperatively oriented cognitions, attitudes, and actions through which team members convert member inputs to team outputs. (p. 181)

6 Small Group Research 00(0)

Carson et al. (2007) further highlighted the fact that team processes such as cooperation or helping, unlike shared leadership, did not involve any active kind of influence. They also distinguished between shared leadership and team cognition constructs such as shared mental models because “the former [shared leadership] concerns collective influence, whereas the latter concerns collective cognition” (p. 1221). Accordingly, we argue that shared leadership and team processes constitute distinct phenomena, with the former often being conceptualized as an input to team processes and teamwork (Aubé, Rousseau, & Brunelle, 2017).

SAL and Research Team Effectiveness

Effective teams manage to balance different criteria (e.g., productivity and member satisfaction), without ever “completely sacrificing any one to achieve the others” (Hackman & Wageman, 2005, p. 272). Team effective- ness, accordingly, has been conceived as a multidimensional construct (e.g., M. S. Cole, Carter, & Zhang, 2013; Kozlowski & Bell, 2003; Mathieu, Maynard, Rapp, & Gilson, 2008), distinguishing, for example, among perfor- mance output, viability, and satisfaction of members’ needs (Hackman, 1987). Here, we focus on three indicators of team effectiveness: an objective indicator of publication quality as reflected by impact factor, a measure of team performance as perceived by team members, and a measure of team satisfaction.

Based on extant theory and prior research, we anticipate that SAL will be positively related to research team effectiveness. That is, we expect that the synergistic effects of authentic leadership that have been shown to operate in dyads and teams (Banks et al., 2016; Gardner et al., 2011) will emerge within research teams as SAL, enabling members to coordinate and build upon one another’s efforts in an open and transparent fashion, thereby producing ele- vated levels of team effectiveness. Hmieleski et al. (2012) indeed showed that SAL in new venture management teams indirectly exerted a positive effect on firm performance, as mediated by team-level positive affective tone. Additional support comes from Hannah et al. (2011) who found that team leader authenticity influenced the average level of authenticity reflected within teams, which in turn enhanced teamwork and subsequent team productivity.

Authentic leadership theory conceptualizes the dimensions of authentic leadership as covarying, rather than exerting differential effects (Gardner et al., 2005). Thus, although studied separately in the current research for the reasons noted in the introduction, we have no theoretical reason to expect differential effects for particular dimensions of SAL, and thus hypothesize as follows:

Guenter et al. 7

Hypothesis1 (H1): The SAL dimensions of self-awareness, balanced pro- cessing, relational transparency, and internalized moral perspective will be positively associated with research team effectiveness as reflected by impact factor (H1a), perceived team performance (H1b), and team satis- faction (H1c).

Multiple Mediating Pathways

To theoretically ground our mediating mechanisms, we draw from func- tional leadership theory (McGrath, 1962). Functional leadership is a form of team leadership, which is best understood in terms of team needs. That is, the leadership role is “to do, or get done, whatever is not being ade- quately handled for group needs” (McGrath, 1962, p. 5). Insofar as anyone (inside or outside the team) takes on the responsibility of satisfying a team need, they are a leader of the team (Morgeson, 2005). Thus, multiple indi- viduals may take on a team leadership role when attempting to satisfy a particular need of the team (Morgeson, DeRue, & Karam, 2010). The func- tional perspective of team leadership suggests that leadership can be defined in terms of social problem-solving, rather than describing a prespecified set of behaviors. Therefore, “[a]ny behavior pattern that reflects effective goal- directed action by leader role incumbents would constitute leadership” (Zaccaro et al., 2001, p. 454).

Consistent with this functional problem-solving perspective, Zaccaro et al. (2001) proposed a team effectiveness model according to which func- tional leadership processes influence team effectiveness via four types of mediating mechanisms (i.e., cognitive, motivational, affective, and coordina- tion). Hoch and Kozlowski (2012) consolidated these four team mechanisms into three types of shared team leadership functions—cognitive, affective- motivational, and behavioral. We examined the influence of SAL on team effectiveness in terms of SMM (cognitive), team trust (affective-motiva- tional), and team coordination (behavioral). We label these factors as mediat- ing mechanisms to denote that we study both team processes and emergent states (Marks, Mathieu, & Zaccaro, 2001; see Figure 1).

The SMM concept was introduced to explain how teams effectively achieve coordination in complex, dynamic, and uncertain contexts (Mohammed, Ferzandi, & Hamilton, 2010). SMM (or team mental models) describe the common knowledge that team members hold about their work tasks and/or social processes (Mathieu, Heffner, Goodwin, Salas, & Cannon- Bowers, 2000). We follow Mohammed et al. (2010) and define SMM as “team members’ shared, organized understanding and mental representation of knowledge about key elements of the team’s relevant environment”

8 Small Group Research 00(0)

(p. 879). Team trust is defined as team members’ shared positive expecta- tions about each other’s actions (De Jong & Elfring, 2007, 2010). This implies that team trust describes a shared perception that emerges from individual members’ trust perceptions. Team coordination is about combin- ing the actions of members and synchronizing the different activities to achieve a common goal (Kozlowski & Ilgen, 2006). Team coordination is defined as the “process of orchestrating the sequence and timing of interde- pendent actions” (Marks et al., 2001, p. 363).

We focus on SMM, team trust, and team coordination because of their relevance as mediators between SAL and team effectiveness. As we explain below, these mediators are important in their own right, but they are also meaningful when looked at together (for similar arguments, see De Jong & Elfring, 2010) because each represents one of the three critical team needs— cognitive, affective-motivational, and behavioral—that, according to func- tional leadership theory, must be satisfied for teams to be successful (Morgeson, DeRue, & Karam, 2010). For analytic clarity, we build separate arguments for the different mediating pathways despite our stated use of a multiple mediation model that allows us to simultaneously estimate the rela- tive influence of SMM, team trust, and team coordination.

The mediating role of SMM. SAL should help develop SMM by improving communication and knowledge exchange (Del Carmen Triana, Kirkman, & Wagstaff, 2012; McIntyre & Foti, 2013). Zaccaro et al. (2001) argued that to develop SMM within a team, leaders need to effectively communicate their own mental model of team action to team members. If leader communication

Figure 1. Hypothesized multiple mediation model of SAL and team effectiveness. Note. SAL = shared authentic leadership.

Guenter et al. 9

is effective, the mental models of members should converge (Zaccaro et al., 2001). Following Zaccaro et al. (2001), we suggest a positive influence of SAL on SMM in research teams. SAL involves using information in an open, balanced manner by inviting supportive as well as contradictory input. As such, SAL implies that members who take on leadership responsibilities are likely to share more information with others (D’Innocenzo et al., 2016), introduce perspectives that otherwise would remain unheard (Dionne, Sayama, Hao, & Bush, 2010), and enhance team knowledge distribution (McIntyre & Foti, 2013). Members can thus learn more from others’ explana- tions, adapt their own mental models, and develop a shared understanding (Levesque, Wilson, & Wholey, 2001).

We expect the components of SAL to jointly facilitate SMM. First, by sharing leadership in ways reflective of deepened self-awareness, SAL facili- tates SMM by building a “collective awareness of the team’s motives, strengths, and weaknesses” (Lyubovnikova, Legood, Turner, & Mamakouka, 2015). Second, by seeking positive and negative feedback from team mem- bers—indicative of balanced processing—SAL benefits SMM as teams develop “an accurate sense of their work and their relationships to superiors, followers, and peers” (Diddams & Chang, 2012, p. 597). Third, relational transparency, which involves “expressing one’s thoughts and emotions openly and sharing information that often is personal with others” (Guenter, Schreurs, van Emmerik, & Sun, 2017, p. 53), serves to promote the common understanding that characterizes SMM. Fourth, the more leaders are commit- ted to core ethical values (i.e., internalized moral perspective), the more tan- gible their moral standards are, thus providing normative guideline for followers. In a team where SAL is given, the mutual expression of members’ core ethical values promotes a common understanding of the ethical behav- iors accepted (see Bouckenooghe, Zafar, & Raja, 2015).

Empirical research seems to support this view. McIntyre and Foti (2013) found partial support for a positive link between shared leadership and men- tal model similarity in student teams. Solansky (2008) found that student teams with shared leadership developed greater metaknowledge about who in the team knew what. In addition, Marks, Zaccaro, and Mathieu (2000) found that teams formed more SMM when leaders provided members with detailed information, and Dionne et al. (2010) found that participative leadership pro- moted SMM development. Given that Solansky (2008) and McIntyre and Foti (2013) focused on student teams, questions about the generalizability of these findings remain. To advance insights that go beyond these findings, we propose that SAL is positively associated with SMM in research teams.

SMM should enable teams to similarly interpret information and antici- pate others’ needs and actions, ultimately benefiting team effectiveness.

10 Small Group Research 00(0)

Maynard and Gilson (2014), for instance, proposed mental model conver- gence to positively associate with virtual team performance. Field and labo- ratory studies demonstrate a positive link between SMM and team effectiveness (DeChurch & Mesmer-Magnus, 2010). Although studied less often, SMM have also been associated with team satisfaction and team via- bility (e.g., Resick, Dickson, Mitchelson, Allison, & Clark, 2010). Thus, we hypothesize as follows:

Hypothesis 2 (H2): SMM will mediate the positive relationship between the SAL dimensions of self-awareness, balanced processing, relational transparency, and internalized moral perspective, and team effectiveness as reflected by impact factor (H2a), perceived team performance (H2b), and team satisfaction (H2c).

The mediating role of team trust. A second reason why SAL benefits research teams is that it promotes trust. When engaging in SAL, members use the influence granted by others in a balanced manner, thereby creating trust and ultimately benefiting team functioning. Gardner et al. (2005) argued that members will develop trust in leaders who make decisions in a manner that is self-reflective, transparent, and grounded in a moral perspective. Authentic leaders strive toward openness and truthfulness in relationships and engage in balanced processing instead of forcing their own agenda (Ilies et al., 2005). Followers can rely on past experiences to predict leaders’ reactions, and, thus, are more willing to trust the leader in the future (Clapp-Smith, Vogelgesang, & Avey, 2009). The more truthful and transparent leaders are in their decision making, the more insights followers gain about the leaders’ expectations and values, which consolidates trust (Gardner et al., 2005).

Wang et al. (2014) indeed suggested that team trust emerges when mem- bers “work toward a common goal and lead each other’s behaviors” (p. 185). Boies, Lvina, and Martens (2010) argued that it is the sharing of responsibil- ity with respect to work and each other that provides a foundation for trust. Drescher et al. (2014) suggested that acts of mutual influence provide mem- bers opportunities to interact and exchange resources. Such exchanges signal trustworthiness, and, if successful and repeated, help to build trust (Bergman, Rentsch, Small, Davenport, & Bergman, 2012). Thus, by sharing leadership responsibilities, members accept each other’s influence, and, if that influence is used responsibly, trust is likely to emerge. We thus propose that when team members share leadership responsibilities in an authentic manner, the out- comes tend to be positive.

This is because of the way that the four components of SAL benefit team trust. Leaders who are self-aware know their own strengths and weaknesses

Guenter et al. 11

and how they affect others, which helps them to connect with and build trust with others. Second, balanced processing helps generate trust at the team level because leaders, by questioning their own positions, signal that they value open communication; by signaling openness to one another, team members who share leadership responsibilities promote feelings of safety and trust (for similar arguments, see Guenter et al., 2017). Third, SAL instills trust among fellow team members through transparent communication of their true values and preferences (e.g., Ilies et al., 2005; Norman, Avolio, & Luthans, 2010). Fourth, guided by internalized moral standards, team mem- bers—who assume and share the leader role—make decisions that are consis- tent with their personal values; the more consistent a focal leader’s words and deeds, the more trust will develop among fellow team members (e.g., Palanski, Kahai, & Yammarino, 2011). In support of these arguments, Bergman et al. (2012) and Drescher et al. (2014) found a positive association between shared leadership and team trust in student teams. Clapp-Smith et al. (2009), using data from retail store employees, revealed that trust in manage- ment partially mediated a positive link between authentic leadership and sales growth.

Team trust, again, has been shown to enhance team effectiveness. De Jong, Dirks, and Gillespie (2016) established a significant positive associa- tion between team trust and team performance. Across 112 independent stud- ies, they found team trust to have an above-average effect on team performance. Breuer, Hüffmeier, and Hertel (2016) provided additional information with regard to attitudinal outcomes. Across 15 virtual team stud- ies, Breuer et al. (2016) found a strong positive association between team trust and team attitudes (e.g., team satisfaction). Thus, consistent with earlier work, we hypothesize as follows:

Hypothesis 3 (H3): Team trust will mediate the positive relationship between the SAL dimensions of self-awareness, balanced processing, relational transparency, and internalized moral perspective, and team effectiveness as reflected by impact factor (H3a), perceived team perfor- mance (H3b), and team satisfaction (H3c).

The mediating role of team coordination. SAL also enhances research team effectiveness because it is likely to benefit team coordination. SAL should help teams orchestrate the timing and sequencing of member actions, making teams more effective (Wang et al., 2014). Zaccaro et al. (2001) described team coordination as one of the main mechanisms explaining why team lead- ership increases team effectiveness. They argued that to ensure team effec- tiveness, leaders help teams identify and integrate the resources and

12 Small Group Research 00(0)

contributions of members. To the extent that members share the responsibil- ity for doing so, and to the extent that members share information about their thoughts and emotions while limiting the display of inappropriate feelings (i.e., reflective of relational transparency), teams should find it easier to coor- dinate their actions, especially when working on complex tasks characterized by high uncertainty.

We expect to find positive relationships between the four SAL compo- nents and team coordination. First, by leading each other in a self-aware manner, team members are likely to gain a “greater awareness of, and atten- tion to, their own processes” (Rico, Sánchez-Manzanares, Gil, & Gibson, 2008, p. 179). This, in turn, should help them anticipate problems, adapt behavior, and improve coordination. Second, team leaders who exhibit bal- anced processing of information, by exploring alternate approaches and questioning dominant views of the situation, are less likely to overlook issues and problems that may hamper coordination (e.g., Eid, Mearns, Larsson, Laberg, & Johnsen, 2012). Third, relational authenticity implies that leaders promote open communication, “especially among employees whose voices may have otherwise been absent” (Boekhorst, 2015, p. 246), which, in turn, should facilitate coordination among team members. In con- trast, if members do not share relevant information, it is more likely that coordination will fail because members will be more prone to act on con- flicting information. Fourth, the behaviors of leaders with an internalized moral perspective become more predictable because they seek consistency in intentions and actions (Spitzmuller & Ilies, 2010); as team members who assume and share leadership roles become more predictable, it becomes easier for fellow team members to proactively adapt their behavior, which will facilitate team coordination.

Although empirical research into the association between shared leader- ship and team coordination is largely absent, findings from Hoch, Pearce, and Welzel (2010) do suggest a positive correlation between shared leadership and team coordination. Team coordination, in turn, is critical for team perfor- mance because it helps to “prevent rework, redundancy, and performance gaps” (Morgeson, Lindoerfer, & Loring, 2010, p. 298). LePine, Piccolo, Jackson, Mathieu, and Saul (2008), using meta-analytic techniques, showed that coordination is positively related with team performance and team satis- faction. Taken together, these arguments suggest that team coordination explains the association between SAL and research team effectiveness.

Hypothesis 4 (H4): Team coordination will mediate the positive relation- ship between the SAL dimensions of self-awareness, balanced processing, relational transparency, and internalized moral perspective, and team

Guenter et al. 13

effectiveness as reflected by impact factor (H4a), perceived team perfor- mance (H4b), and team satisfaction (H4c).

Method

Sample

We tested our hypotheses on author teams of scientific articles in peer- reviewed management journals (see Berka, Olien, Rogelberg, Rupp, & Thornton, 2014; Rupp, Thornton, Rogelberg, Olien, & Berka, 2014). We selected a sample of journals representative of the quality of research done in management science; specifically, we selected journals based on their clas- sification in the 2012 Social Science Citation Index (SSCI) under the cate- gory of management/business. Six journals were classified as high SSCI, six were classified as medium SSCI, and five were classified as low according to the SSCI. The journals included were, for example, Academy of Management Journal, Journal of Business and Psychology, and European Management Journal.1 For each journal, we selected all articles labeled online first, that is, accepted, peer reviewed articles (but not editorials, invited contributions, or commentaries) that were available online but not yet assigned to a volume/ issue. We included recently accepted articles to minimize memory bias in respondents.

We only included articles coauthored by at least two scholars, consistent with extant team definitions (Kozlowski & Ilgen, 2006; Salas, Dickinson, Converse, & Tannenbaum, 1992). While two-person teams may seem small, we decided to include these teams (rather than only teams with three or more members) because of the nature of the teams under investigation, that is, scientific research teams. The average number of coauthors in the social sci- ences, in 2000, was roughly two (Wuchty et al., 2007); the average number of coauthors in management journals such as the Academy of Management Journal was slightly below 2.5 in 2008 (Certo, Sirmon, & Brymer, 2010). Apart from these statistics, it is difficult to imagine how the breakthroughs of Kahneman and Tversky (1979), March and Simon (1958), or Locke and Latham (1990)—to name only a few—could have been achieved without teamwork.

We collected data in two waves in 2013 and 2014. In the 2013 wave, we included articles published online first over a period ranging from May 2011 to December 2013. In the 2014 wave, we included online first articles ranging from April 2013 to April 2014. The second wave was necessary because the sample distribution of journal impact factors from Wave 1 was skewed (61.5% of the articles had a journal impact factor of 2.71 and higher). Thus, in Wave 2,

14 Small Group Research 00(0)

we purposively oversampled articles from lower tier journals (based on impact factor). Because these journals tended to have a far shorter time period from paper acceptance to publication in a volume/issue than higher tier journals, the number of articles available “online first” at these journals was lower. Hence, for the lower tier journals, we also included manuscripts from the two most recently published issues in Wave 2. In all, we invited 2,745 authors (Wave 1: 1,670 authors; Wave 2: 1,075 authors) to complete the survey.

We received 716 surveys (Wave 1: 489; Wave 2: 227) resulting in a response rate of 26.1%. After deletion of respondents with missing data, we were left with 628 respondents. We omitted teams with less than two respon- dents to be able to assess interrater reliability and agreement before data aggregation (Schippers, Den Hartog, & Koopman, 2007). We were left with complete data for 317 authors (Wave 1: 233, Wave 2: 84) from 142 research teams. Although the response rate obtained is lower than desired, it is not unusual for studies found in the organizational sciences (Baruch & Holtom, 2008). Moreover, it is not the response rate per se that is a concern, but the representativeness of the sample, that is critical to avoiding potential nonre- sponse bias (Rogelberg & Stanton, 2007).

We followed advice by Rogelberg and Stanton (2007) to assess nonresponse bias. First, we compared individual characteristics of authors included in the analysis with those excluded (i.e., age, gender, H-index, country of affiliation, and impact factor). This analysis revealed no significant differences for age, gen- der, and H-index. Differences existed for country of affiliation and impact factor. The ratio of authors from North America was higher among those included ver- sus those excluded from the analysis. In addition, the impact factor was signifi- cantly higher among those included as compared with those authors excluded from the analysis. This indicates that although we purposively oversampled arti- cles from lower tier journals in the second wave of data collection, more versus less successful teams were still more likely to respond to our survey.

The average size of each author team was 3.45 (SD = 1.28); the average number of respondents per author team was 2.23 (SD = 0.51). There were no significant team-level differences between the two data collection waves for the main model variables except for SMM (team-level mean values were higher in Wave 2 [6.07] than Wave 1 [5.76]). As intended, the impact factor in Wave 2 (3.20) was lower than in Wave 1 (4.17). Table 1 provides further details on the respondents who participated in our study.

Procedure

We contacted authors via e-mail with a link to an online survey (contact information used was publicly available). We mentioned the title of the

Guenter et al. 15

Table 1. Sample Characteristics.

Variables Sample (N = 317)

Gender Men 198 (62.5%) Women 114 (36%) Age 18-30 52 (16.4%) 31-40 121 (38.2%) 41-50 82 (25.9%) 51-60 39 (12.3%) >60 21 (6.6%) Author location United States/Canada 219 (69.1%) Europe 65 (20.5%) Asia/Pacific 28 (8.8%) Other 2 (0.6%) Academic or business professional Academic 304 (95.9%) Business professional 10 (3.2%) Author discipline Management 252 (79.5%) Other 51 (16.1%) Management subdiscipline Organizational behavior 115 (36.3%) Strategic management 42 (13.2%) Human resources 31 (9.8%) Entrepreneurship 24 (7.6%) Organization theory 16 (5%) Operations management 1 (0.3%) Academic appointment PhD student 35 (11%) Postdoc 8 (2.5%) Lecturer 7 (2.2%) Assistant professor 85 (26.8%) Associate professor 75 (23.7%) Full professor 67 (21.1%) Other 6 (1.9%)

Note. Numbers sometimes do not add up to 100% because not all respondents provided the respective information.

16 Small Group Research 00(0)

selected scientific article and instructed authors to complete the survey with this publication in mind. In accordance with the institutional review board’s protocols, we informed participants that participation was voluntary. We also told authors that they would receive a US$10 Amazon gift certificate if they completed the survey, and followed up by providing these certificates to all participants.

Measures

We measured all constructs using established scales. Unless indicated other- wise, we only made minor changes in wording to increase item clarity or to adapt the instrument to the study context (e.g., substituting the word “group” with “team” and rephrasing questions in the past tense). A stem that reminded authors to answer all questions, keeping the author team and their experi- ences during the publication process in mind, preceded each scale. To assess the internal consistency of each scale (Cronbach’s alpha), we used the item mean for each team to align internal consistency information with the team level of analysis used for the substantive tests (Chen, Bliese, & Mathieu, 2005; Mathieu & Schulze, 2006).

Team effectiveness. We measured team effectiveness in terms of impact fac- tor, perceived team performance, and team satisfaction. With regard to impact factor, we used the 2013 impact factor of the journal where the article was published. This indicator expresses the “average number of citations to jour- nal articles in their first two years following publication” (Rynes, 2007, p. 1273). The impact factor, although debated, is a widely “accepted standard for quantifying the scholarly influence of journals” (Simsek, Heavey, & Jan- sen, 2013, p. 1374). Next, to measure perceived team performance, we used a four-item scale taken from Gibson, Cooper, and Conger (2009). A sample item reads, “This team was effective.” Responses were given on a 7-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (very inaccurate) to 7 (very accurate). Cronbach’s alpha for this scale was .86. Finally, we used a five-item scale taken from Behfar, Peterson, Mannix, and Trochim (2008) to measure team satisfaction. A sample item reads, “To what extent were you satisfied working with this team?” Responses were given on a seven-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (totally), with the exception of one item for which the scale ranged from 1 (never) to 7 (always). Cronbach’s alpha for this scale was .91.

SAL. We measured SAL in terms of self-awareness, relational transparency, balanced processing, and internalized moral perspective using the 16 items

Guenter et al. 17

developed by Walumbwa et al. (2008). For the overall scale, Cronbach’s α was .94. Responses were given on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (frequently, if not always). We applied a referent-shift compo- sition logic (Chan, 1998) to change the stem from “my leader” to “members of this team.” This was necessary to capture authentic leadership behaviors as they apply to the whole research team instead of only to an individual leader.2

Following the recommendations of Banks et al. (2016), Gardner et al. (2011), and Neider and Schriesheim (2011), we conducted analyses at the SAL component level.3 We used four items to measure self-awareness (Cronbach’s α = .88), such as, “Members of this team sought feedback to improve interactions with others” and “accurately described how others view their capabilities.” We used five items to measure relational transparency (Cronbach’s α = .88), such as, “Members of this team admitted mistakes when they were made” and “said exactly what they meant.” We used three items to measure balanced processing (Cronbach’s α = .74), such as, “Members of this team solicited views that challenged their deeply held positions” and “listened carefully to different points of view before coming to conclusions.” To mea- sure internalized moral perspective, we used four items (Cronbach’s α = .83), such as, “Members of this team demonstrated beliefs that were consistent with actions” and “made decisions based on their core values.”

SMM. We used a five-item scale taken from Fransen, Kirschner, and Erkens (2011) to measure SMM (Cronbach’s α = .91). A sample item reads, “Shortly after the start, this team had a common understanding of the task we had to handle.” Responses were given on a 7-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).

Team trust. We used three items from Walumbwa, Luthans, Avey, and Oke (2011) to measure trust (Cronbach’s α = .87).4 The three items were “How much did members of your team trust each other?” “Were your team mem- bers truthful and honest?” and “How comfortable did members of your team feel delegating to other team members?” Responses were given on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much).

Team coordination. We used a three-item scale taken from DeChurch and Haas (2008) to measure team coordination (Cronbach’s α = .90). A sample item reads, “The team effectively coordinated member actions.” Responses were given on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (to a great extent).

Control variables. First, we expected team familiarity to positively relate to research team effectiveness (Huckman, Staats, & Upton, 2009). Kotha et al.

18 Small Group Research 00(0)

(2013) argued that prior collaboration experience (akin to familiarity) helps research teams to develop shared understanding and coordination routines, ultimately reducing coordination costs and benefiting team effectiveness. We used a three-item scale taken from Gevers, van Eerde, and Rutte (2009) to measure team familiarity (Cronbach’s α = .83). A sample item reads, “In my team, we were acquainted with each other’s way of working.” Responses were given on a five-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (disagree com- pletely) to 5 (agree completely). Second, we included team size because we expected team size to associate with research team effectiveness. Prior research, although inconsistent, provides some support for this view (Stew- art, 2006). Third, in alignment with team research more generally (Stewart & Barrick, 2000), we expected task interdependence to associate with research team effectiveness. We used a three-item scale taken from Campion, Medsker, and Higgs (1993) to measure task interdependence (Cronbach’s α = .79). A sample item reads, “I was not able to accomplish my tasks without informa- tion or materials from other members of my team.” Responses were given on a 7-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Fourth, we controlled for the team’s past research productivity because prior author success may predict future success (Judge, Cable, Colbert, & Rynes, 2007). We used the H-index to reflect publication productivity and impact. We retrieved the H-index per author from public databases (e.g., Sco- pus) and calculated the team average.

Data Analysis Approach

We used structural equation modeling (SEM) to test our multiple mediation model (see Preacher & Hayes, 2008). To test our hypotheses, we adopted Mplus 7.2 using maximum-likelihood estimation (Muthén & Muthén, 1998- 2014). First, we used confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to test the conver- gent and discriminant validity of our team-level model variables (i.e., four dimensions of SAL, SMM, trust, coordination, and three indicators of research team effectiveness). We assessed the model factor structure at the team level to align our psychometric analyses with the level of theory for the team constructs involved (Hitt, Beamish, Jackson, & Mathieu, 2007; Mathieu & Chen, 2011). We performed three distinct analyses because we used three indicators of team effectiveness. For all models, we set the first loading for each factor to 1 to define the latent factor scale. To assess model fit, we used fit indices recommended by Hu and Bentler (1999) including the compara- tive fix index (CFI), root mean squared error of approximation (RMSEA), and standardized root mean squared residual (SRMR). For sake of complete- ness, we also report the chi-square statistic. RMSEA values of .05 or less

Guenter et al. 19

indicate a good model fit and values of .08 and less a reasonable model fit (Browne & Cudeck, 1993). SRMR values close to .08 indicate acceptable model fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999). CFI values of .95 and higher signify a good model fit and values in the range of .90 and .95 indicate acceptable fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999).

Second, to formally test our model, we constructed bias-corrected boot- strapped confidence intervals for testing specific indirect effects (Lau & Cheung, 2012). In using bootstrapping, we circumvented problems that limit ordinary least square regression analysis such as uncorrected measurement errors (D. A. Cole & Preacher, 2013) and nonnormality in the sample distri- bution (Preacher & Hayes, 2008). Other advantages of bootstrapping described by Lau and Cheung (2012) are that the effects of control variables can be implemented easily and that the significance of the specific indirect effects is tested. Overall, bootstrapping is a robust method for testing specific mediation effects in a latent variable model.

Results

Aggregation Statistics

To assess whether aggregation of our main model variables was viable, we relied on a set of complementary statistics using the multilevel package in R (version 3.0.2) (Bliese, 2013). We followed James, Demaree, and Wolf’s (1984) example for calculating agreement indices for multi-item scales (rWG(j)) and single-item scales (rWG). The agreement values for our main model variables were satisfactory, ranging from .74 to .91 (see Table 2) (LeBreton & Senter, 2008). We also computed intraclass correlation coeffi- cients (i.e., ICC(1) and ICC(2) values) to assess interrater reliability. ICC(1) values reflect the proportion of variance in individuals’ ratings that is due to team membership (Bliese & Ployhart, 2002). The ICC(1) values ranged from .11 to .36 (see Table 2), and are largely consistent with those typically reported in management field research (Aguinis, Gottfredson, & Culpepper, 2013). These values indicate that a medium to large proportion of nonindependence in the data can be explained by team membership (LeBreton & Senter, 2008).

ICC(2) values express the reliability of group mean values. ICC(2) values ranged from .22 to .56 (see Table 2), which is below the critical cut-off of .60 suggested by Glick (1985). ICC(2) values are a direct function of team size (Bliese, 1998), which is why we modeled how ICC(2) values would change if we had sampled larger teams with more respondents (see Brown & Trevino, 2006). To simulate the ICC(2) values for teams of 5 and 10 respondents, we used the Spearman-Brown formula (Bliese, 2000). Results showed ICC(2)

20 Small Group Research 00(0)

values would have been higher, ranging from .38 to .74, and .55 to .85, respectively. Consistent with other scholars (Chen & Bliese, 2002) and because we found evidence for within-team agreement, demonstrated team- level effects, and used referent-shift items appropriate with higher-unit phe- nomena (Chan, 1998), we progressed with the analysis. Still, given our rather low ICC(2) values, it may be somewhat difficult to identify team-level rela- tionships (Bliese, 2000).

Measurement Model: Convergent and Discriminant Validity

We compared the fit of five nested models, including the hypothesized eight- factor model (i.e., four dimensions of SAL, team trust, coordination, SMM, and team effectiveness) and four alternative models, such as a five-factor model (i.e., identical to the eight-factor model except that the SAL items loaded on a single factor; see Table 3 for details). We also reran the analysis treating SAL as a unidimensional construct (see Table 8). As we discuss in detail below, these findings are not substantially different from our multidi- mensional results.

As presented in Table 3, our hypothesized measurement model provided a better fit to the data than any of the alternative models. We found the hypothesized eight-factor model to have the most acceptable CFI, RMSEA, and SRMR values. Fabrigar, Porter, and Norris (2010) argued that—given moderately favorable conditions—a sample of 200 or more respondents is needed to obtain precise model estimates in SEM. Because our sample size

Table 2. Aggregation Statistics.

Measure rwg(j).uniform ICC(1) ICC(2)

Team performance .91 .26 .44 Team satisfaction .91 .36 .56 SAL .90 .21 .37 Self-awareness .74 .16 .30 Relational transparency .89 .20 .35 Balanced processing .76 .11 .22 Internalized moral perspective

.82 .18 .32

Team coordination .86 .23 .40 Team trust .89 .24 .41 SMM .87 .22 .39

Note. N = 142 teams. ICC = intraclass correlation coefficients; SAL = shared authentic leadership; SMM = shared mental models.

Guenter et al. 21

Table 3. Results of Confirmatory Factor Analysis.

Model χ2 df RMSEA SRMR CFI

Impact factor Eight-factor modela 577.14** (765.72**) 323 (323) .07 (.06) .06 (.05) .91 (.94) Six-factor modelb 877.83** (1751.81**) 336 (336) .11 (.10) .08 (.07) .82 (.82) Five-factor modelc 763.81** (1190.15**) 341 (341) .09 (.08) .07 (.06) .86 (.89) Three-factor modeld 1052.20** (2153.20**) 348 (348) .12 (.11) .09 (.08) .76 (.77) One-factor modele 1360.49** (2724.39**) 350 (350) .14 (.12) .10 (.08) .66 (.70) Team performance Eight-factor modela 709.59** (951.18**) 406 (406) .07 (.06) .06 (.05) .91 (.94) Six-factor modelb 1005.15** (1929.65**) 419 (419 .10 (.09) .08 (.07) .83 (.84) Five-factor modelc 890.54** (1366.28**) 424 (424 .09 (.07) .07 (.05) .87 (.90) Three-factor modeld 1174.72** (2322.37**) 431 (431) .11 (.10) .08 (.07) .79 (.79) One-factor modele 1588.13** (3237.46**) 434 (434) .14 (.12) .10 (.08) .67 (.70) Team satisfaction Eight-factor modela 861.99** (1058.67**) 436 (436) .08 (.06) .06 (.05) .89 (.94) Six-factor modelb 1188.92** (2118.87**) 449 (449) .11 (.09) .08 (.07) .81 (.83) Five-factor modelc 1042.89** (1472.09**) 454 (454) .10 (.07) .07 (.05) .85 (.90) Three-factor modeld 1362.74** (2513.83**) 461 (461) .12 (.10) .09 (.07) .76 (.79) One-factor modele 1813.25** (3532.99**) 464 (464) .14 (.12) .10 (.08) .64 (.69)

Note. N = 142 (and 453) teams. RMSEA = root mean squared error of approximation; SRMR = standardized root mean squared residual; CFI = comparative fix index. aEight factors include self-awareness, balanced processing, relational transparency, moral awareness, SMM, team trust, team coordination, and team performance. bEight-factor model but items measuring SMM, team trust, and team coordination are combined into one factor. cEight-factor model but items measuring self-awareness, balanced processing, relational transparency, and moral awareness are combined into one factor. dEight-factor model but self-awareness, balanced processing, relational transparency, and moral awareness are combined into one factor, and SMM, team trust, and team coordination into another factor. eAll measuring items were combined into one factor. *p < .05. **p <.01.

(N = 142) is lower, we tested the robustness of our results by rerunning all analyses using the teams with one respondent only (Hirschfeld, Cole, Bernerth, & Rizzuto, 2013; Maloney, Johnson, & Zellmer-Bruhn, 2010). We also report these results (based on 453 teams) in Table 3. Results based on this larger sample confirmed the superior relative fit of the eight-factor model.

Descriptive Statistics

Means, standard deviations, and zero-order correlations for our main study variables are presented in Table 4. The dimensions of SAL correlated with the

22

T ab

le 4

. D

es cr

ip tiv

e St

at is

tic s

an d

Bi va

ri at

e C

or re

la tio

ns .

V ar

ia bl

es M

SD 1

2 3

4 5

6 7

8 9

10 11

12 13

14

1 .

Im pa

ct fa

ct or

3. 91

2. 47

2

. T

ea m

p er

fo rm

an ce

5. 94

0. 66

.2 1*

3

. T

ea m

s at

is fa

ct io

n 6.

25 0.

72 .0

2 .6

4* *

4

. SA

L 4.

18 0.

49 .1

2 .6

5* *

.7 0*

*

5 .

SA L:

s el

f-a w

ar en

es s

3. 97

0. 66

.1 0

.5 9*

* .5

6* *

.8 7*

*

6 .

SA L:

r el

at io

na l

tr an

sp ar

en cy

4. 31

0. 47

.0 4

.5 8*

* .6

7* *

.8 6*

* .5

8* *

7 .

SA L:

b al

an ce

d pr

oc es

si ng

4. 12

0. 57

.1 6

.5 5*

* .5

8* *

.9 0*

* .7

6* *

.6 9*

*

8 .

SA L:

in te

rn al

iz ed

m

or al

p er

sp ec

tiv e

4. 29

0. 54

.1 3

.5 6*

* .6

6* *

.9 2*

* .7

1* *

.7 7*

* .7

8* *

9 .

SM M

5. 84

0. 80

.0 1

.6 7*

* .4

9* *

.5 4*

* .5

3* *

.4 3*

* .5

0* *

.4 7*

*

10 .

T ea

m t

ru st

4. 52

0. 49

− .0

2 .6

7* *

.7 6*

* .6

7* *

.5 1*

* .6

7* *

.5 8*

* .6

3* *

.5 2*

*

11 .

T ea

m c

oo rd

in at

io n

4. 32

0. 55

.1 5

.7 4*

* .6

9* *

.6 2*

* .5

6* *

.5 6*

* .5

2* *

.5 6*

* .6

8* *

.6 8*

*

12 .

T ea

m s

iz e

3. 45

1. 28

.0 4

− .1

0 −

.1 8*

− .1

4 −

.1 1

− .1

8* −

.1 4

− .0

6 −

.0 7

− .1

3 −

.1 1

13

. T

ea m

fa m

ili ar

ity 4.

32 0.

54 .0

5 .5

4* *

.5 9*

* .5

7* *

.4 5*

* .5

7* *

.4 9*

* .5

2* *

.4 6*

* .6

2* *

.4 9*

* −

.3 7*

*

14 .

T as

k in

te rd

ep en

de nc

e 5.

80 0.

71 .1

3 .3

7* *

.3 0*

* .2

7* *

.2 3*

* .1

8* .3

2* *

.2 4*

* .4

0* *

.3 6*

* .4

0* *

− .0

7 .2

2* *

15 .

T ea

m h

-in de

x 5.

70 4.

08 .2

1* .2

6* *

.1 7*

.1 3

.1 1

.1 7*

.0 5

.1 1

.1 8*

.1 4

.1 9*

.1 1

.2 0*

.0 8

N ot

e. C

or re

la tio

ns b

as ed

o n

N =

1 42

t ea

m s.

S A

L =

s ha

re d

au th

en tic

le ad

er sh

ip ; S

M M

= S

ha re

d m

en ta

l m od

el s.

*p <

.0 5.

* *p

< .0

1.

Guenter et al. 23

three mediators, which, in turn, were positively associated with perceived team performance and team satisfaction.

Tests of Hypotheses

The results are presented in Tables 5, 6, 7, and 8. Table 5 presents the multiple mediation analysis findings regarding impact factor. Table 6 includes results regarding team performance, and Table 7 displays results regarding team sat- isfaction. These tables include our main results assessing SAL at the compo- nent level. We also included Table 8 which presents findings for the overall construct of SAL.5

Contrary to what we expected in Hypothesis 1a, we found no total effect of SAL on impact factor (see Table 5). However, as expected in Hypotheses 1b and 1c, we found a significant total effect of the SAL dimensions on per- ceived team performance (see Table 6) and team satisfaction (see Table 7). Note that the nonsignificant direct relationship between SAL and impact fac- tor does not tell us whether or not an indirect effect exists. This is because a total effect is the sum of different (direct and indirect) paths of influence, and these different paths may operate in opposite directions. Thus, the effects of multiple paths working in different directions may cancel each other out, resulting in an overall null effect (Hayes, 2009).

SMM as mediator. Hypothesis 2 (a, b, and c) suggested that SMM mediates the relationship between the SAL dimensions and research team effective- ness. Overall, we found only weak support for this hypothesis (see Tables 5, 6, and 7). More specifically, with regard to Hypothesis 2a, we found a nega- tive mediation effect for the pathway toward impact factor, and this relation- ship held across all four dimensions of SAL. The direction of this relationship was opposite to what we expected. For Hypothesis 2b, SMM mediated the association between three of the four dimensions of SAL (except for balanced processing) and perceived team performance. We found no significant sup- port for Hypothesis 2c when testing for the mediation pathway toward team satisfaction.

Team trust as mediator. Hypothesis 3 (a, b, and c) suggested that team trust mediates the association between the SAL dimensions and research team effectiveness. Overall, we found partial support for Hypothesis 3 (see Tables 5, 6, and 7). For Hypothesis 3a, the results on impact factor were largely in the opposite direction of what we had predicted. Specifically, we found an unexpected negative indirect effect through team trust on impact factor for self-awareness, balanced processing, and internalized moral perspective (but

24 Small Group Research 00(0)

Table 5. Multiple Mediation Analysis Using Mplus: Indirect Effects of SAL on Impact Factor.

Distal variable Mediator

Product of coefficients

Bootstrapping bias-corrected

95% CI

Point estimate SE p

Lower limit

Upper limit

Self-awareness SMM −0.87 0.51 .09 −2.33 −0.10 Team trust −1.18 0.64 .06 −3.07 −0.37 Team

coordination 1.16 0.65 .07 0.07 2.66

Total indirect effect −0.89 0.96 .36 −3.57 0.58 Total effect −0.27 0.63 .67 −1.75 0.77 Relational

transparency SMM −0.96 0.62 .12 −2.58 −0.05 Team trust −2.28 1.53 .14 −5.76 0.12

Team coordination

1.81 0.96 .06 0.26 4.08

Total indirect effect −1.43 1.79 .42 −5.22 1.89 Total effect −0.93 1.14 .41 −3.45 1.11 Balanced processing SMM −1.80 1.23 .14 −5.50 −0.56 Team trust −3.47 2.39 .15 −10.51 −1.23 Team

coordination 0.55 2.02 .79 −3.33 2.49

Total indirect effect −4.73 4.31 .27 −16.17 −0.45 Total effect 0.83 1.53 .59 −1.34 4.19 Internalized moral

perspective SMM −1.11 0.75 .14 −3.20 −0.19 Team trust −3.07 2.07 .14 −8.76 −0.84

Team coordination

1.19 0.96 .22 −0.27 3.43

Total indirect effect −2.99 2.54 .24 −9.39 0.18 Total effect 0.04 1.00 .97 −2.21 1.87

Note. N = 142 teams. Bootstrap sample size = 1,000. Coefficients in boldface indicate mediation. SAL = shared authentic leadership; CI = confidence interval; SMM = shared mental models.

not relational transparency). When predicting perceived team performance (Hypothesis 3b), we only found a mediation effect for self-awareness. Inter- estingly, if we had studied SAL as a unidimensional construct, we would have missed this significant effect (see Table 8). As predicted in Hypothesis 3c, team trust did mediate the positive association between all four dimen- sions of SAL and team satisfaction.

Guenter et al. 25

Team coordination as mediator. Hypothesis 4 (a, b, and c) suggested that team coordination mediates the association between the SAL dimensions and team effectiveness. Overall, results were largely in line with Hypothesis 4 (see Tables 5, 6, and 7). As expected in Hypothesis 4a, we found a significant positive mediation effect for self-awareness and relational transparency via team coordination on impact factor. We found no effects for balanced

Table 6. Multiple Mediation Analysis Using Mplus: Indirect Effects of SAL on Team Performance.

Distal variable Mediator

Product of coefficients

Bootstrapping bias-corrected

95% CI

Point estimate SE p

Lower limit

Upper limit

Self-awareness SMM 0.23 0.11 .04 0.06 0.51 Team trust 0.23 0.11 .04 0.03 0.49 Team

coordination 0.37 0.15 .01 0.14 0.74

Total indirect effect 0.82 0.22 .00 0.51 1.42 Total effect 0.95 0.22 .00 0.58 1.43 Relational

transparency SMM 0.35 0.15 .02 0.12 0.73 Team trust 0.36 0.26 .17 −0.12 0.86

Team coordination

0.56 0.23 .02 0.23 1.11

Total indirect effect 1.28 0.39 .00 0.70 2.21 Total effect 1.45 0.33 .00 0.93 2.22 Balanced processing SMM 0.25 0.17 .15 −0.05 0.64 Team trust 0.18 0.31 .56 −0.50 0.68 Team

coordination 0.41 0.27 .13 −0.11 0.88

Total indirect effect 0.84 0.51 .10 −0.37 1.61 Total effect 1.53 0.57 .01 0.82 3.04 Internalized moral

perspective SMM 0.31 0.14 .03 0.10 0.70 Team trust 0.31 0.25 .21 −0.17 0.80

Team coordination

0.50 0.28 .07 0.19 1.24

Total indirect effect 1.12 0.44 .01 0.54 2.12 Total effect 1.30 0.34 .00 0.80 2.14

Note. N = 142 teams. Bootstrap sample size = 1,000. Coefficients in boldface indicate mediation. SAL = shared authentic leadership; CI = confidence interval; SMM = Shared mental models.

26 Small Group Research 00(0)

processing and internalized moral perspective. Concerning Hypothesis 4b, we found overall support for the mediation pathway leading toward perceived team performance (except for balanced processing). As expected in Hypoth- esis 4c, team coordination mediated the association between the four SAL dimensions and team satisfaction. Note that our conclusions concerning team

Table 7. Multiple Mediation Analysis Using Mplus: Indirect Effects of SAL on Team Satisfaction.

Distal variable Mediator

Product of coefficients

Bootstrapping bias-corrected

95% CI

Point estimate SE p

Lower limit

Upper limit

Self-awareness SMM −0.08 0.10 .41 −0.31 0.08 Team trust 0.43 0.13 .00 0.24 0.81 Team

coordination 0.31 0.15 .04 0.09 0.68

Total indirect effect 0.66 0.21 .00 0.37 1.24 Total effect 0.67 0.16 .00 0.43 1.07 Relational

transparency SMM −0.10 0.13 .44 −0.39 0.10 Team trust 0.75 0.28 .01 0.35 1.41

Team coordination

0.45 0.21 .03 0.13 0.92

Total indirect effect 1.10 0.35 .00 0.57 1.95 Total effect 1.20 0.27 .00 0.80 1.85 Balanced processing SMM −0.17 0.19 .37 −0.66 0.09 Team trust 0.61 0.31 .05 0.20 1.44 Team

coordination 0.39 0.39 .32 0.02 1.06

Total indirect effect 0.83 0.64 .19 0.06 2.34 Total effect 1.07 0.38 .01 0.64 2.09 Internalized moral

perspective SMM −0.12 0.16 .43 −0.48 0.08 Team trust 0.55 0.24 .02 0.19 1.18

Team coordination

0.36 0.20 .06 0.07 0.78

Total indirect effect 0.79 0.35 .02 0.21 1.57 Total effect 1.13 0.26 .00 0.74 1.75

Note. N = 142 teams. Bootstrap sample size = 1,000. Coefficients in boldface indicate mediation. SAL = shared authentic leadership; CI = confidence interval; SMM = shared mental models.

Guenter et al. 27

coordination would have differed if we had studied SAL as a unidimensional construct. Specifically, we would have concluded that there is an overall pos- itive mediation effect of SAL through team coordination on impact factor (see Table 8).

Taken together, our multiple mediation results provide some support for our hypotheses, but appear to be more complex than expected. The effects that we established hold above and beyond the influence that team familiar- ity, team size, task interdependence, and the team’s past research productivity have on research team effectiveness.6

Discussion

We proposed that SAL would be positively related to the effectiveness of research teams. By sharing authentic leadership influence, teams were

Table 8. Multiple Mediation Analysis Using Mplus: Indirect Effects of SAL on Team Effectiveness.

Outcome variable Mediator

Product of coefficients

Bootstrapping bias-corrected

95% CI

Point estimate SE p

Lower limit

Upper limit

Impact factor SMM −1.19 0.68 .08 −2.86 −0.17 Team trust −2.67 1.08 .01 −5.04 −0.73 Team coordination 1.64 0.91 .07 0.05 3.61 Total indirect effect −2.22 1.33 .10 −4.72 0.44 Total effect −0.19 1.00 .85 −2.52 1.68 Team performance SMM 0.37 0.16 .02 0.12 0.78 Team trust 0.36 0.21 .08 −0.02 0.79 Team coordination 0.58 0.23 .01 0.23 1.14 Total indirect effect 1.31 0.34 .00 0.80 2.14 Total effect 1.57 0.33 .00 0.97 2.28 Team satisfaction SMM −0.11 0.13 .38 −0.38 0.11 Team trust 0.73 0.23 .00 0.35 1.21 Team coordination 0.46 0.20 .02 0.14 0.94 Total indirect effect 1.07 0.30 .00 0.61 1.81 Total effect 1.26 0.26 .00 0.90 1.86

Note. N = 142 teams. Bootstrap sample size = 1,000. Coefficients in boldface indicate mediation. Input variable for all models is SAL. SAL = shared authentic leadership; CI = confidence interval; SMM = shared mental model.

28 Small Group Research 00(0)

expected to become more effective because of cognitive (i.e., SMM), affec- tive-motivational (i.e., team trust), and behavioral (i.e., team coordination) mechanisms. Our results suggest that SAL operates through different mediat- ing pathways, albeit to varying degrees. The results indicate that team coor- dination was the primary mediating mechanism accounting for the relationship between SAL and research team effectiveness. SMM and team trust served as secondary pathways between SAL and research team effectiveness, but in less consistent ways. Research teams high on trust and SMM felt more suc- cessful (and more satisfied), but were in fact less successful in publishing in high-impact management journals. Our results also revealed that the SAL dimensions were differentially and indirectly related with research team effectiveness and that these differences would have remained masked if we had treated SAL as a unidimensional construct.

Our findings on the link between SAL and research team effectiveness were mixed. As expected, SAL was positively associated with perceived team performance and team satisfaction. Contrary to expectations, we did not find a link between SAL and impact factor. This suggests that SAL scholars may come to different conclusions depending upon whether they focus on objective or subjective indicators of team effectiveness. This is problematic because measurement-dependent differences in findings may be misread as substantive issues. Thus, scholars may need to use more com- prehensive sets of indicators to fully ascertain the implications of SAL in research teams.

We obtained tenuous support for the view that SAL transmits its effects via SMM. Although we found the expected indirect relationship for perceived team performance (except for balanced processing), no relationship was revealed for team satisfaction and a negative indirect relationship was found for impact factor. Team cognition research shows that SMM benefit team performance (DeChurch & Mesmer-Magnus, 2010), and our findings for per- ceived team performance support this view. Yet our results for impact factor (suggesting a negative indirect effect) contradict this beneficial view of SMM. Possibly, members assumed their cognitive processes to be shared when in fact they were not (e.g., false consensus; Ross, Greene, & House, 1977). Specifically, teams might have been affected by low degrees of cross- understanding, that is, “the extent to which group members have accurate understandings of one another’s mental models” (Huber & Lewis, 2010, p. 7). Alternatively, pluralistic ignorance, a phenomenon in which members do not share their actual beliefs, perceptions, or feelings because they mistakenly assume that their privately held positions differ from other team members (Miller & Prentice, 1994), may have been operative. By not openly sharing opinions and ideas with other team members because of false consensus and/

Guenter et al. 29

or pluralistic ignorance, pluralistic arrogance—a term coined by Randolph- Seng and Norris (2011) to describe a maladaptive form of SMM that is simi- lar to groupthink (e.g., Goncalo, Polman, & Maslach, 2010)—may have emerged. Thus, we suggest that key parts of designing and conducting high- quality research (e.g., disagreeing with others and discussing those disagree- ments) may have been missing and may have influenced teams despite members’ SMM. Skilton and Dooley (2010) explained why shared cognition may actually restrict creativity in research teams. Specifically, they argued that the more rigid and uniform SMM are, the less varied and novel the ideas are that team members generate and select. The less novel the ideas are that research teams generate, the less likely it is for them to change or challenge existing concepts, models, or theories, which, in turn, reduces the likelihood of publishing in top-tier journals.

We did find partial support for the team trust mediation path. As expected, we found an indirect relationship for team satisfaction, which reinforces find- ings from Costa (2003). However, we found little evidence for an indirect effect on perceived team performance, which is inconsistent with earlier research (Costa, 2003) and requires further clarification. Most noticeable for SAL theory is our finding of a negative indirect path for impact factor (except for relational transparency). This result is inconsistent with those obtained by Drescher et al. (2014), which might be due to differences in context. Whereas Drescher et al. (2014) focused on teams engaged in strategic decision mak- ing, we studied teams working toward scientific discoveries. Performance in research teams, more than in strategy teams, depends upon divergent thinking and creative problem solving, and thus, hinges upon the open exchange of diverse and potentially conflicting ideas. When operating under high levels of trust, team members may favor conformity and avoid constructive debate, which may restrict the chance of getting published in premier scientific jour- nals (e.g., Mok & Morris, 2010). This argument may also help explain why Braun, Peus, Weisweiler, and Frey (2013), studying 30 academic teams working at German universities, found no association between team trust and research output. Overall, our findings suggest that there might be circum- stances under which team trust becomes somewhat ineffective (see also Tsai, Chi, Grandey, & Fung, 2012).

Our hypothesis regarding team coordination was largely supported. We found support for the expected indirect path for team satisfaction, perceived team performance (except for balanced processing), and impact factor (except for balanced processing and internalized moral perspective). That we found support across different measures of team effectiveness speaks to the robustness of the team coordination relationships. This makes our study the first to support the hypothesis that SAL benefits teams by enabling them

30 Small Group Research 00(0)

to better coordinate their activities. We find that it is specifically the team’s self-awareness and relational transparency that enhances team coordina- tion—as the two pathways for the other SAL components (balanced process- ing and internalized moral perspective) were insignificant. By leading each other in an open, nondefensive manner (i.e., relational transparency) and in ways grounded in self-knowledge (i.e., self-awareness), research teams can integrate the scientific input of members more effectively. That these effects hold above and beyond the influence of a team member’s past research pro- ductivity (measured via the team’s H-index) is intriguing and highlights the role of teamwork in scientific research, which too often is misconceived as purely individual work.

Theoretical Implications

First, our findings contribute to authentic leadership theory. Research has focused on studying the effects of an individual authentic leader instead of conceiving authentic leadership as a team quality and as a set of distributed leadership functions. Consider, for example, Banks and colleauges’ (2016) study, which meta-analytically reviewed the consequences of authentic lead- ership, uncovering 100 samples with a total of more than 25,000 individuals. They needed to exclude one study from the review (i.e., Hmieleski et al., 2012) because it stood out in that it focused on “AL [authentic leadership] of entire teams instead of individual leaders” (Banks et al., 2016, p. 637). We do not call into question the merits of studying individual authentic leadership, but suggest that studies into SAL can complement existing findings. By pro- viding evidence of the links between SAL and research team effectiveness, especially through team coordination, our study directs attention to the team- based, shared view of leadership as an important angle from which to inves- tigate authentic leadership.

By borrowing functional leadership theory from the team literature, we developed novel hypotheses on the mediating mechanisms of SAL. That we identify team coordination—a behavioral mechanism—as the most important mediating mechanism challenges some implicit assumptions in SAL research and authentic leadership theory. Some exceptions aside (e.g., Hannah et al., 2011), research on authentic leadership has mainly studied emotional-motivational and cognitive mediators (see Avolio & Walumbwa, 2014). For instance, Hmieleski et al. (2012) found that group affective tone mediates the association of SAL with new venture perfor- mance. Similarly, Clapp-Smith et al. (2009) showed that trust in manage- ment explained some beneficial effects of authentic leadership on store performance. Our research suggests these findings may be incomplete in

Guenter et al. 31

that behavioral mechanisms may work in parallel with these emotional- motivational and cognitive mediators.

That we found multiple mediating mechanisms to act in parallel also con- tributes to the broader literature on shared leadership. Drescher et al. (2014) found team trust to mediate the association between shared leadership and team performance, but suggested that other mechanisms may work along- side team trust (e.g., team cognition), because “trust is but one facet of the social functioning of the group” (p. 780). Nicolaides et al. (2014), similarly, called for research studying mediators other than team confidence—which was the focus of their meta-analytic investigation. Our study addressed this call, thereby deepening our understanding of shared leadership and the fun- damental processes underlying its effects. Such knowledge is essential because it helps develop interventions for practice that specifically target the variables (e.g., team coordination) in the mediating process (MacKinnon & Fairchild, 2009).

Instead of treating SAL as a unidimensional construct (see Hmieleski et al., 2012), we followed recommendations from Banks et al. (2016), Gardner et al. (2011), and Neider and Schriesheim (2011) to examine SAL at the component level. We found that the dimensions of SAL have differential effects on team effectiveness. Although we would not have obtained substantially different results had we studied SAL as a unidimensional construct, our multidimen- sional findings are more nuanced and provide novel insights that we otherwise would have missed. For instance, only self-awareness and relational transpar- ency—but not balanced processing and internalized moral perspective—were associated with impact factor through coordination. If we had assessed SAL as a unidimensional construct, we would have inferred an overall indirect rela- tionship instead. Thus, our analysis highlights the need to more systematically examine the effects of authentic leadership at the component level.

Finally, our study contributes to the field of the science of team science (Börner et al., 2010; Fiore, 2008; Rupp et al., 2014; Salazar et al., 2012). Over the last decade, our understanding of the challenges of team science has greatly increased (see Börner et al., 2010). Still, there is little research into the kind of leadership and teamwork that may help scientific teams cope with challenges. Rupp et al. (2014) thus called for research into team factors that may explain manuscript success/quality. Our study responds to these calls and addresses the question of how to execute science in teams by explaining how shared leadership stimulates high-quality research output (i.e., through team coordination) and by pointing to the limits of SAL. That is, by increas- ing shared cognition and team trust, SAL may also limit creativity and inno- vation in research teams, making it more difficult for them to publish in the most esteemed peer-reviewed management journals.

32 Small Group Research 00(0)

Limitations and Future Research

One limitation of our study is that we relied upon self-reports (except for impact factor) to measure our model variables, raising common method con- cerns. Based on the expectations for assessing the potential effects of com- mon method variance proposed by Conway and Lance (2010), we believe that common method concerns are unlikely to be a major issue in our data for the following reasons: (a) Team members held the best insights regarding the focal constructs under study, (b) we used established scales and demonstrated convergent/discriminant validity of the model constructs, and (c) we miti- gated threats of method effects proactively and found our results were con- firmed when controlling for social desirability bias—using five items from the Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding (Paulhus, 1991).7

Second, we cannot rule out reverse causality completely because we mea- sured most of our model variables simultaneously. As such, hindsight bias on the part of participants may be an alternate explanation for the results found; however, given that participants were asked about their perceptions of the team in multiple ways, hindsight bias may have been somewhat lessened in the current study (Sanna & Schwarz, 2003). Furthermore, one could specu- late that performance perceptions influence the degree to which teams share their leadership responsibilities. Although conceivable, it seems unlikely that perceived team performance (or team satisfaction) drives leadership, and extant research supports the direction of influence we tested (e.g., Hmieleski et al., 2012). Still, future cross-lagged analysis is needed to robustly establish the direction of influence.

Third, we purposefully sampled teams who had published their work because we wanted a somewhat objective measure of scholarly team success (i.e., publication quality). By sampling only teams that managed to publish their work, we may have restricted the variance in our variables. Such range restriction has been found to deflate rather than inflate correlations relative to what one would expect to find if no restriction of range occurred (e.g., Berry, Cullen, & Meyer, 2014; Cable & Kay, 2012). Thus, we might have underes- timated the correlations, relative to what the associations may be in an unre- stricted sample (Berry et al., 2014). Although this does not reduce confidence in the significant effects found in this study, the nonsignificant correlations need to be interpreted with caution. Specifically, we found SMM to not medi- ate the link between SAL and team satisfaction. Before disqualifying SMM as a mediator, further unrestricted testing seems warranted.

Fourth, consistent with the approach taken by Hmieleski et al. (2012) to oper- ationalize SAL, we used a referent shift approach where the stem for the Authentic Leadership Questionnaire was changed from “my leader” to “members of this

Guenter et al. 33

team.” However, Van Mierlo, Vermunt, and Rutte (2009) note, the use of such composition methods to operationalize group-level constructs can be problem- atic. Here, it is important to recognize that we follow their recommendations to employ aggregation statistics (e.g., ICC(1) and ICC(2)) and factor analysis to assess the reliability and construct validity of the resultant group-level measure. Nevertheless, we advocate for a more complete assessment of the construct validity of the SAL measure following the more extensive procedures for multi- level theory building and scale development by Van Mierlo et al. (2009).

Fifth, the number of mediating mechanisms accounted for in our study was limited, and more complex multiple models are conceivable. Scholars may want to complicate our model by distinguishing different types of SMM (Maynard & Gilson, 2014) and/or between explicit and implicit coordination mechanisms (Rico et al., 2008). Future studies should also explore boundary conditions of SAL (e.g., Hoch & Kozlowski, 2012) and assess whether differ- ent team functions are context specific. Possibly, emotional and cognitive team functions are more relevant in nonresearch teams. For instance, in new venture and strategy making teams, SAL may operate primarily through affect rather than coordination (see Drescher et al., 2014; Hmieleski et al., 2012). To test this possibility, future research should build and test more com- plex conditional indirect effects models within this literature.

Practical Implications

Our findings have several practical implications. First, our finding that SAL associates with team effectiveness while controlling for team size and past research productivity underscores the fact that research team success is not only a matter of composition, but is also dependent upon leadership and teamwork. This finding aligns with that of Woolley, Gerbasi, Chabris, Kosslyn, and Hackman (2008) who found that team analytic work depends both on individual expertise and collaboration. Thus, our results should moti- vate research teams to invest in leadership and team-building activities.

Second, our results indicate that SAL may have some unintended negative side effects, as indicated by the negative indirect effect of SAL on impact factor mediated via team trust and SMM. Thus, management scholars may need to remind themselves that a healthy amount of skepticism (toward each other’s ideas and contributions) might increase, instead of decrease, their publication prospects (Langfred, 2004). Coauthors may also need to refresh their author team’s cognitive structures by restructuring teams periodically (Skilton & Dooley, 2010).

Finally, coauthors may judge their team to be effective, but may nonethe- less fail to publish in top-tier management outlets. Author teams should be

34 Small Group Research 00(0)

cautious about relying on their own judgment as a leading indicator of future publication success, and instead invite critical feedback from subject matter experts who are not on the author team (Skilton & Dooley, 2010). However, it is important that author teams select reviewers who are indeed critical; otherwise, teams may end up with positively biased feedback that may do more harm than good (see Berka et al., 2014). As the design of a research study cannot be altered once the data have been collected, it seems important to receive such critical feedback early in the research process, instead of when a manuscript summarizing the findings is available.

Conclusion

As workplaces grow increasingly complex and dynamic, organizations are placing a premium on teams, teamwork, and complementary leadership pro- cesses. While this is true across a wide array of work contexts, to date, little attention has been devoted to the importance of teamwork in research teams. We begin filling this void by studying SAL—a form of leadership that is particularly well suited for fostering the requisite levels of collaboration and coordination that scientific teamwork requires. Our results suggest team coordination is the primary mediating mechanism, and SMM and team trust serve as secondary mechanisms, whereby SAL is related to perceived team performance and satisfaction. However, while research teams high on trust and SMM expressed greater satisfaction with the team and perceived their performance to be high, they were surprisingly less successful in publishing their work in high-impact journals. Possibly, it is that research teams high in SAL experience pluralistic arrogance—a form of groupthink—that causes them to think they are performing better than objective outcomes suggest. Our results thus hint not only at the benefits of SAL, but also highlight poten- tial adverse consequences that heretofore have not been considered. Moreover, our findings suggest that when it comes to research teams, the importance of team coordination for team effectiveness may be key to helping the team reach their desired outcomes.

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge receiving funding for this research from the Southern Management Association as part of the 2011 Research Incubator.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Guenter et al. 35

Funding

The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This research received funding from the Southern Management Association as part of the 2011 Research Incubator.

Notes

1. The journals included (in alphabetical order) were Academy of Management Journal, Academy of Management Review, Asia Pacific Business Review, Business Horizons, Canadian Journal of Administrative Sciences, Corporate Governance: An International Review, European Journal of International Management, European Management Journal, International Business Review, International Journal of Human Resource Management, Journal of Business and Psychology, Journal of Management, Journal of Management Inquiry, Journal of Management Studies, Management Learning, Organization Science, and The Leadership Quarterly.

2. Note that this shift in perspective (from a single leader to the team) does not alter the nature of the authentic leadership behaviors displayed and that the logic of our approach aligns with Hmieleski, Cole, and Baron (2012), one of the very few studies to explore shared authentic leadership (SAL). Although competing measurement methods exist (e.g., network density measures), meta- analytic findings show that measurement differences are unlikely to have any substantial implications for shared leadership and its effects (Wang, Waldman, & Zhang, 2014).

3. Note that we are not permitted to depict all SAL items because they form part of a copyright-protected measurement scale. Still, we wish to mention that the items on self-awareness primarily reflect a sense of self-evaluation and willing- ness to learn about oneself and about how one’s behavior affects others. As such, this operationalization is congruent with its conceptual definition.

4. We obtained evidence for the validity of the team trust scale using complete data obtained from teams where we had only one respondent per team (n = 325 sin- gle-respondent teams). These data were not used in our main analysis. The valid- ity assessment was necessary because the three items were taken from an article (Walumbwa, Luthans, Avey, & Oke, 2011) that was retracted after our data col- lection was completed (Retraction published 2014, Journal of Organizational Behavior, 35, p. 746). Cronbach’s alpha was .82.

5. Note that we do not report effect size measures for mediation effects. This is because commonly used effect size measures “have not yet been extended for use in models involving multiple mediators” (Preacher & Kelley, 2011, p. 108). Wen and Fan (2015), more recently, expressed similar concerns.

6. We considered the possibility that research team effectiveness may depend on the mix of scholars in terms of research experience and productivity. Thus, we reran our analyses controlling for team-level diversity in research produc- tivity (approximated via the H-index); we distinguished between diversity as

36 Small Group Research 00(0)

separation and as disparity (Harrison & Klein, 2007). For 93% of the indirect effects models that we tested, results remained essentially unchanged (i.e., the direction and significance of the relationships was confirmed). As per a review- er’s suggestion, we also assessed whether the length of the publication process and number of rejections at other journals might have had an impact on the per- ceived effectiveness of the team (i.e., team performance and team satisfaction). To this end, we controlled for the number of journals that had rejected the paper prior to acceptance. We found that both the nature and significance of our results remained unchanged. We thus have confidence that our results do not depend on the length of the publication process. Results of this analysis are available upon request from the first author.

7. Findings are available upon request from the first author.

References

Aguinis, H., Gottfredson, R. K., & Culpepper, S. A. (2013). Best-practice recommen- dations for estimating cross-level interaction effects using multilevel modeling. Journal of Management, 39, 1490-1528. doi:10.1177/0149206313478188

Aubé, C., Rousseau, V., & Brunelle, E. (2017). Flow experience in teams: The role of shared leadership. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1037/ocp0000071

Avolio, B. J., & Gardner, W. L. (2005). Authentic leadership development: Getting to the root of positive forms of leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 16, 315-338. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2005.03.001

Avolio, B. J., & Walumbwa, F. O. (2014). Authentic leadership theory, research and practice: Steps taken and steps that remain. In D. V. Day (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Leadership and Organizations (pp. 331-356). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Banks, G. C., McCauley, K. D., Gardner, W. L., & Guler, C. E. (2016). A meta-analytic review of authentic and transformational leadership: A test for redundancy. The Leadership Quarterly, 27, 634-652. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2016.02.006

Baruch, Y., & Holtom, B. C. (2008). Survey response rate levels and trends in organiza- tional research. Human Relations, 61, 1139-1160. doi:10.1177/0018726708094863

Behfar, K. J., Peterson, R. S., Mannix, E. A., & Trochim, W. M. (2008). The critical role of conflict resolution in teams: A close look at the links between conflict type, conflict management strategies, and team outcomes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 170-188. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.93.1.170

Bergman, J. Z., Rentsch, J. R., Small, E. E., Davenport, S. W., & Bergman, S. M. (2012). The shared leadership process in decision-making teams. The Journal of Social Psychology, 152, 17-42. doi: 10.1080/00224545.2010.538763

Berka, G., Olien, J., Rogelberg, S. G., Rupp, D. E., & Thornton, M. A. (2014). An inductive exploration of manuscript quality and publication success in small research teams. Journal of Business and Psychology, 29, 725-731. doi: 10.1007/ s10869-014-9373-6

Guenter et al. 37

Berry, C. M., Cullen, M. J., & Meyer, J. M. (2014). Racial/ethnic subgroup differ- ences in cognitive ability test range restriction: Implications for differential valid- ity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99, 21-37. doi:10.1037/a0034376

Bliese, P. D. (1998). Group size, ICC values, and group-level correlations: A simulation. Organizational Research Methods, 1, 355-373. doi:10.1177/109442819814001

Bliese, P. D. (2000). Within-group agreement, non-independence, and reliability: Implications for data aggregation and analysis. In K. J. Klein & S. W. J. Kozlowski (Eds.), Multilevel theory, research, and methods in organizations: Foundations, extensions, and new directions (pp. 349-381). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Bliese, P. D. (2013). Multilevel Modeling in R (2.5). Retrieved from https://cran.r- project.org/doc/contrib/Bliese_Multilevel.pdf

Bliese, P. D., & Ployhart, R. E. (2002). Growth modeling using random coefficient models: Model building, testing, and illustrations. Organizational Research Methods, 5, 362-387. doi:10.1177/109442802237116

Boekhorst, J. A. (2015). The role of authentic leadership in fostering workplace inclu- sion: A social information processing perspective. Human Resource Management, 54, 241-264. doi:10.1002/hrm.21669

Boies, K., Lvina, E., & Martens, M. L. (2010). Shared leadership and team perfor- mance in a business strategy simulation. Journal of Personnel Psychology, 9, 195-202. doi:10.1027/1866-5888/a000021

Börner, K., Contractor, N., Falk-Krzesinski, H. J., Fiore, S. M., Hall, K. L., Keyton, J., . . .Uzzi, B. (2010). A multi-level systems perspective for the science of team science. Science Translational Medicine, 2(49), 1-5. doi:10.1126/scitrans- lmed.3001399

Bouckenooghe, D., Zafar, A., & Raja, U. (2015). How ethical leadership shapes employees’ job performance: The mediating roles of goal congruence and psychological capital. Journal of Business Ethics, 129, 251-264. doi: 10.1007/ s10551-014-2162-3

Braun, S., Peus, C., Weisweiler, S., & Frey, D. (2013). Transformational leadership, job satisfaction, and team performance: A multilevel mediation model of trust. The Leadership Quarterly, 24, 270-283. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2012.11.006

Breuer, C., Hüffmeier, J., & Hertel, G. (2016). Does trust matter more in virtual teams? A meta-analysis of trust and team effectiveness considering virtuality and documentation as moderators. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101, 1151-1177. doi: 10.1037/apl0000113

Brown, M. E., & Trevino, L. K. (2006). Socialized charismatic leadership, values con- gruence, and deviance in work groups. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 954-962. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.91.4.954

Browne, M., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136-162). Newbury Park, CA: SAGE.

Bruns, H. (2013). Working alone together: Coordination in collaboration across domains of expertise. Academy of Management Journal, 56, 62-83. doi:10.5465/ amj.2010.0756

38 Small Group Research 00(0)

Cable, D. M., & Kay, V. S. (2012). Striving for self-verification during organiza- tional entry. Academy of Management Journal, 55, 360-380. doi:10.5465/ amj.2010.0397

Campion, M. A., Medsker, G. J., & Higgs, A. C. (1993). Relations between work group characteristics and effectiveness: Implications for designing effective work groups. Personnel Psychology, 46, 823-847. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6570.1993. tb01571.x

Carson, J. B., Tesluk, P. E., & Marrone, J. A. (2007). Shared leadership in teams: An investigation of antecedent conditions and performance. Academy of Management Journal, 50, 1217-1234. doi:10.2307/AMJ.2007.20159921

Certo, S. T., Sirmon, D. G., & Brymer, R. A. (2010). Competition and scholarly productivity in management: Investigating changes in scholarship from 1988 to 2008. Academy of Management Learning & Education, 9, 591-606. doi:10.5465/ amle.2010.56659878

Chan, D. (1998). Functional relations among constructs in the same content domain at different levels of analysis: A typology of composition models. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 234-246. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.83.2.234

Chen, G., & Bliese, P. D. (2002). The role of different levels of leadership in predict- ing self-and collective efficacy: Evidence for discontinuity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 549-556. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.87.3.549

Chen, G., Bliese, P. D., & Mathieu, J. E. (2005). Conceptual framework and sta- tistical procedures for delineating and testing multilevel theories of homology. Organizational Research Methods, 8, 375-409. doi:10.1177/1094428105280056

Clapp-Smith, R., Vogelgesang, G. R., & Avey, J. B. (2009). Authentic leadership and positive psychological capital: The mediating role of trust at the group level of analysis. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies, 15, 227-240. doi:10.1177/1548051808326596

Cole, D. A., & Preacher, K. J. (2013). Manifest variable path analysis: Potentially serious and misleading consequences due to uncorrected measurement error. Psychological Methods, 19, 300-315. doi:10.1037/a0033805

Cole, M. S., Carter, M. Z., & Zhang, Z. (2013). Leader-team congruence in power distance values and team effectiveness: The mediating role of procedural justice climate. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98, 962-973. doi:10.1037/a0034269

Conway, J. M., & Lance, C. E. (2010). What reviewers should expect from authors regarding common method bias in organizational research. Journal of Business and Psychology, 25, 325-334. doi: 10.1007/s10869-010-9181-6

Costa, A. C. (2003). Work team trust and effectiveness. Personnel Review, 32, 605- 622. doi:10.1108/00483480310488360

DeChurch, L. A., & Haas, C. D. (2008). Examining team planning through an episodic lens: Effects of deliberate, contingency, and reactive planning on team effective- ness. Small Group Research, 39, 542-568. doi:10.1177/1046496408320048

DeChurch, L. A., & Mesmer-Magnus, J. R. (2010). The cognitive underpinnings of effective teamwork: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 32-53. doi:10.1037/a0017328

Guenter et al. 39

De Jong, B. A., Dirks, K. T., & Gillespie, N. (2016). Trust and team performance: A meta-analysis of main effects, moderators, and covariates. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101, 1134-1150. doi: 10.1037/apl0000110

De Jong, B. A., & Elfring, T. (2007). Mediated and differential effects of trust on team performance: A study of ongoing work teams. Academy of Management Best Paper Proceedings, 2007(1), 1-6.

De Jong, B. A., & Elfring, T. (2010). How does trust affect the performance of ongo- ing teams? The mediating role of reflexivity, monitoring, and effort. Academy of Management Journal, 53, 535-549. doi: 10.5465/AMJ.2010.51468649

Del Carmen Triana, M., Kirkman, B. L., & Wagstaff, M. F. (2012). Does the order of face-to-face and computer-mediated communication matter in diverse project teams? An investigation of communication order effects on minority inclusion and participation. Journal of Business and Psychology, 27, 57-70. doi: 10.1007/ s10869-011-9232-7

Diddams, M., & Chang, G. C. (2012). Only human: Exploring the nature of weakness in authentic leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 23, 593-603. doi: 10.1016/j. leaqua.2011.12.010

D’Innocenzo, L., Mathieu, J. E., & Kukenberger, M. R. (2016). A meta-analysis of different forms of shared leadership–team performance relations. Journal of Management, 42, 1964-1991. doi:10.1177/0149206314525205

Dionne, S. D., Sayama, H., Hao, C., & Bush, B. J. (2010). The role of leadership in shared mental model convergence and team performance improvement: An agent-based computational model. The Leadership Quarterly, 21, 1035-1049. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2010.10.007

Drescher, M. A., Korsgaard, M. A., Welpe, I. M., Picot, A., & Wigand, R. T. (2014). The dynamics of shared leadership: Building trust and enhancing performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99, 771-783. doi:10.1037/a0036474

Eid, J., Mearns, K., Larsson, G., Laberg, J. C., & Johnsen, B. H. (2012). Leadership, psychological capital and safety research: Conceptual issues and future research questions. Safety Science, 50, 55-61. doi: 10.1016/j.ssci.2011.07.001

Fabrigar, L. R., Porter, R. D., & Norris, M. E. (2010). Some things you should know about structural equation modeling but never thought to ask. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 20, 221-225. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2010.03.003

Fiore, S. M. (2008). Interdisciplinarity as teamwork: How the science of teams can inform team science. Small Group Research, 39, 251-277. doi:10.1177/1046496408317797

Fransen, J., Kirschner, P. A., & Erkens, G. (2011). Mediating team effectiveness in the context of collaborative learning: The importance of team and task awareness. Computers in Human Behavior, 27, 1103-1113. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2010.05.017

Gardner, W. L., Avolio, B. J., Luthans, F., May, D. R., & Walumbwa, F. O. (2005). “Can you see the real me?” A self-based model of authentic leader and fol- lower development. The Leadership Quarterly, 16, 343-372. doi: 10.1016/j. leaqua.2005.03.003

Gardner, W. L., Cogliser, C. C., Davis, K. M., & Dickens, M. P. (2011). Authentic leadership: A review of the literature and research agenda. The Leadership Quarterly, 22, 1120-1145. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2011.09.007

40 Small Group Research 00(0)

Gevers, J. M. P., van Eerde, W., & Rutte, C. G. (2009). Team self-regulation and meeting deadlines in project teams: Antecedents and effects of temporal con- sensus. European Journal of Work & Organizational Psychology, 18, 295-321. doi:10.1080/13594320701693217

Gibson, C. B., Cooper, C. D., & Conger, J. A. (2009). Do you see what we see? The complex effects of perceptual distance between leaders and teams. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 62-76. doi:10.1037/a0013073

Glick, W. H. (1985). Conceptualizing and measuring organizational and psychologi- cal climate: Pitfalls in multilevel research. Academy of Management Review, 10, 601-616. doi:10.5465/AMR.1985.4279045

Goncalo, J. A., Polman, E., & Maslach, C. (2010). Can confidence come too soon? Collective efficacy, conflict and group performance over time. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 113, 13-24. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2010.05.001

Guenter, H., Schreurs, B., van Emmerik, I., & Sun, S. (2017). What does it take to break the silence in teams: Authentic leadership and/or proactive follower- ship? Applied Psychology: An International Review, 66, 49-77. doi:10.1111/ apps.12076

Hackman, J. R. (1987). The design of work teams. In J. Lorsch (Ed.), Handbook of organizational behavior (pp. 315-342). New York, NY: Prentice Hall.

Hackman, J. R., & Wageman, R. (2005). A theory of team coaching. Academy of Management Review, 30, 269-287. doi:10.5465/AMR.2005.16387885

Hannah, S. T., Walumbwa, F. O., & Fry, L. W. (2011). Leadership in action teams: Team leader and members’ authenticity, authenticity strength, and team outcomes. Personnel Psychology, 64, 771-802. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.2011.01225.x

Harrison, D. A., & Klein, K. J. (2007). What’s the difference? Diversity constructs as separation, variety, or disparity in organizations. Academy of Management Review, 32, 1199-1228. doi:10.5465/AMR.2007.26586096

Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analy- sis in the new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76, 408-420. doi:10.1080/03637750903310360

Hirschfeld, R. R., Cole, M. S., Bernerth, J. B., & Rizzuto, T. E. (2013). Voluntary survey completion among team members: Implications of noncompliance and missing data for multilevel research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98, 454- 468. doi:10.1037/a0031909

Hitt, M. A., Beamish, P. W., Jackson, S. E., & Mathieu, J. E. (2007). Building theoretical and empirical bridges across levels: Multilevel research in man- agement. Academy of Management Journal, 50, 1385-1399. doi:10.5465/ AMJ.2007.28166219

Hmieleski, K. M., Cole, M. S., & Baron, R. A. (2012). Shared authentic leader- ship and new venture performance. Journal of Management, 38, 1476-1499. doi:10.1177/0149206311415419

Hoch, J. E., & Kozlowski, S. W. (2012). Leading virtual teams: Hierarchical lead- ership, structural supports, and shared team leadership. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99, 390-403. doi:10.1037/a0030264

Guenter et al. 41

Hoch, J. E., Pearce, C. L., & Welzel, L. (2010). Is the most effective team leadership shared? The impact of shared leadership, age diversity, and coordination on team performance. Journal of Personnel Psychology, 9, 105-116. doi:10.1027/1866- 5888/a000020

Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance struc- ture analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 1-55. doi:10.1080/10705519909540118

Huber, G. P., & Lewis, K. (2010). Cross-understanding: Implications for group cogni- tion and performance. Academy of Management Review, 35, 6-26. doi:10.5465/ amr.2010.45577787

Huckman, R. S., Staats, B. R., & Upton, D. M. (2009). Team familiarity, role experi- ence, and performance: Evidence from Indian software services. Management Science, 55, 85-100. doi:10.1287/mnsc.1080.0921

Ilies, R., Morgeson, F. P., & Nahrgang, J. D. (2005). Authentic leadership and eudae- monic well-being: Understanding leader-follower outcomes. The Leadership Quarterly, 16, 373-394. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2005.03.002

James, L. R., Demaree, R. G., & Wolf, G. (1984). Estimating within-group interrater reliability with and without response bias. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69, 85-98. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.69.1.85

Jones, B. F., Wuchty, S., & Uzzi, B. (2008). Multi-university research teams: Shifting impact, geography, and stratification in science. Science, 322, 1259-1262. doi:10.1126/science.1158357

Jonsen, K., Butler, C. L., Mäkelä, K., Piekkari, R., Drogendijk, R., Lauring, J., . . .Zander, L. (2012). Processes of international collaboration in management research: A reflexive, autoethnographic approach. Journal of Management Inquiry, 22, 394-413. doi: 10.1177/1056492612464429

Judge, T. A., Cable, D. M., Colbert, A. E., & Rynes, S. L. (2007). What causes a man- agement article to be cited—Article, author, or journal? Academy of Management Journal, 50, 491-506. doi:10.5465/AMJ.2007.25525577

Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica: Journal of the Econometric Society, 47, 263-291. doi:10.2307/1914185

Kernis, M. H. (2003). Toward a conceptualization of optimal self-esteem. Psychological Inquiry, 14, 1-26. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1401_01

Kernis, M. H., & Goldman, B. M. (2006). A multicomponent conceptualization of authenticity: Theory and research. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 283-357. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(06)38006-9

Kotha, R., George, G., & Srikanth, K. (2013). Bridging the mutual knowledge gap: Coordination and the commercialization of university science. Academy of Management Journal, 56, 498-524. doi:10.5465/amj.2010.0948

Kozlowski, S. W., & Bell, B. S. (2003). Work groups and teams in organizations. In W. Borman, D. Ilgen & R. Klimoski (Eds.), Handbook of Psychology: Industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 333-375). London, England: Wiley. doi:10.1002/0471264385.wei1214

42 Small Group Research 00(0)

Kozlowski, S. W., & Ilgen, D. R. (2006). Enhancing the effectiveness of work groups and teams. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 7(3), 77-124. doi:10.1111/j.1529-1006.2006.00030.x

Langfred, C. W. (2004). Too much of a good thing? Negative effects of high trust and individual autonomy in self-managing teams. Academy of Management Journal, 47, 385-399. doi:10.2307/20159588

Lau, R. S., & Cheung, G. W. (2012). Estimating and comparing specific mediation effects in complex latent variable models. Organizational Research Methods, 15, 3-16. doi:10.1177/1094428110391673

LeBreton, J. M., & Senter, J. L. (2008). Answers to 20 questions about interrater reli- ability and interrater agreement. Organizational Research Methods, 11, 815-852. doi:10.1177/1094428106296642

LePine, J. A., Piccolo, R. F., Jackson, C. L., Mathieu, J. E., & Saul, J. R. (2008). A meta-analysis of teamwork processes: Tests of a multidimensional model and relationships with team effectiveness criteria. Personnel Psychology, 61, 273- 307. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.2008.00114.x

Leroy, H., Anseel, F., Gardner, W. L., & Sels, L. (2015). Authentic leadership, authen- tic followership, basic need satisfaction, and work role performance: A cross-level study. Journal of Management, 41, 1677-1697. doi:10.1177/0149206312457822

Levesque, L. L., Wilson, J. M., & Wholey, D. R. (2001). Cognitive divergence and shared mental models in software development project teams. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 22, 135-144. doi: 10.1002/job.87

Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting & task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Luthans, F., & Avolio, B. J. (2003). Authentic leadership: A positive developmental approach. In K. S. Cameron, J. E. Dutton & R. E. Quinn (Eds.), Positive organi- zational scholarship (pp. 241-261). San Francisco, CA: Berrett-Koehler.

Lyubovnikova, J., Legood, A., Turner, N., & Mamakouka, A. (2015). How authentic leadership influences team performance: The mediating role of team reflexivity. Journal of Business Ethics, 141, 59-70. doi: 10.1007/s10551-015-2692-3

MacKinnon, D. P., & Fairchild, A. J. (2009). Current directions in mediation analysis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 16-20. doi:10.1111/ j.1467-8721.2009.01598.x

Maloney, M. M., Johnson, S. G., & Zellmer-Bruhn, M. E. (2010). Assessing group- level constructs under missing data conditions: A Monte Carlo simulation. Small Group Research, 41, 281-307. doi:10.1177/1046496410363829

March, J. G., & Simon, H. A. (1958). Organizations. Oxford, UK: Wiley. Marks, M. A., Mathieu, J. E., & Zaccaro, S. J. (2001). A temporally based framework

and taxonomy of team processes. Academy of Management Review, 26, 356-376. doi:10.5465/AMR.2001.4845785

Marks, M. A., Zaccaro, S. J., & Mathieu, J. E. (2000). Performance implications of leader briefings and team-interaction training for team adaptation to novel environments. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 971-986. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.85.6.971

Guenter et al. 43

Mathieu, J. E., & Chen, G. (2011). The etiology of the multilevel paradigm in management research. Journal of Management, 37, 610-641. doi:10.1177/ 0149206310364663

Mathieu, J. E., Heffner, T. S., Goodwin, G. F., Salas, E., & Cannon-Bowers, J. A. (2000). The influence of shared mental models on team process and performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 273-283. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.85.2.273

Mathieu, J. E., Maynard, M. T., Rapp, T., & Gilson, L. (2008). Team effectiveness 1997-2007: A review of recent advancements and a glimpse into the future. Journal of Management, 34, 410-476. doi:10.1177/0149206308316061

Mathieu, J. E., & Schulze, W. (2006). The influence of team knowledge and for- mal plans on episodic team process-performance relationships. Academy of Management Journal, 49, 605-619. doi:10.5465/AMJ.2006.21794678

Maynard, M. T., & Gilson, L. L. (2014). The role of shared mental model devel- opment in understanding virtual team effectiveness. Group & Organization Management, 39, 3-32. doi:10.1177/1059601113475361

McGrath, J. E. (1962). Leadership behavior: Some requirements for leadership train- ing. Washington, DC: U.S. Civil Service Commission.

McIntyre, H. H., & Foti, R. J. (2013). The impact of shared leadership on teamwork mental models and performance in self-directed teams. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 16, 46-57. doi:10.1177/1368430211422923

Miller, D. T., & Prentice, D. A. (1994). Collective errors and errors about the collective. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 541-550. doi:10.1177/0146167294205011

Milojević, S. (2014). Principles of scientific research team formation and evolu- tion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111, 3984-3989. doi:10.1073/pnas.1309723111

Mohammed, S., Ferzandi, L., & Hamilton, K. (2010). Metaphor no more: A 15-year review of the team mental model construct. Journal of Management, 36, 876- 910. doi:10.1177/0149206309356804

Mok, A., & Morris, M. W. (2010). Asian-Americans’ creative styles in Asian and American situations: Assimilative and contrastive responses as a function of bicultural identity integration. Management and Organization Review, 6, 371- 390. doi:10.1111/j.1740-8784.2010.00190.x

Morgeson, F. P. (2005). The external leadership of self-managing teams: Intervening in the context of novel and disruptive events. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 497-508. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.90.3.497

Morgeson, F. P., DeRue, D. S., & Karam, E. P. (2010). Leadership in teams: A func- tional approach to understanding leadership structures and processes. Journal of Management, 36, 5-39. doi:10.1177/0149206309347376

Morgeson, F. P., Lindoerfer, D., & Loring, D. J. (2010). Developing team leadership capability. In E. V. Velsor, C. McCauley & M. Ruderman (Eds.), The Center for Creative Leadership handbook of leadership development (Vol. 122, pp. 285- 312). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

44 Small Group Research 00(0)

Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998-2014). Mplus version 7. Los Angeles, CA: Author.

Neider, L. L., & Schriesheim, C. A. (2011). The Authentic Leadership Inventory (ALI): Development and empirical tests. The Leadership Quarterly, 22, 1146- 1164. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2011.09.008

Nicolaides, V. C., LaPort, K. A., Chen, T. R., Tomassetti, A. J., Weis, E. J., Zaccaro, S. J., & Cortina, J. M. (2014). The shared leadership of teams: A meta-analysis of proximal, distal, and moderating relationships. The Leadership Quarterly, 25, 923-942. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2014.06.006

Norman, S. M., Avolio, B. J., & Luthans, F. (2010). The impact of positivity and transparency on trust in leaders and their perceived effectiveness. The Leadership Quarterly, 21, 350-364. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2010.03.002

Palanski, M. E., Kahai, S. S., & Yammarino, F. J. (2011). Team virtues and perfor- mance: An examination of transparency, behavioral integrity, and trust. Journal of Business Ethics, 99, 201-216. doi: 10.1007/s10551-010-0650-7

Paulhus, D. L. (1991). Measurement and control of response bias. In J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psy- chological attitudes (Vol. 1, pp. 17-59). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Pearce, C. L., & Manz, C. C. (2005). The new silver bullets of leadership: The importance of self-and shared leadership in knowledge work. Organizational Dynamics, 34, 130-140. doi: 10.1016/j.orgdyn.2005.03.003

Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879-891. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.3.879

Preacher, K. J., & Kelley, K. (2011). Effect size measures for mediation models: Quantitative strategies for communicating indirect effects. Psychological Methods, 16, 93-115. doi: 10.1037/a0022658

Randolph-Seng, B., & Norris, J. I. (2011). Cross-understanding in groups: How to “cross over” without “dying.” Academy of Management Review, 36, 420-422. doi:10.5465/amr.2010.0177

Resick, C. J., Dickson, M. W., Mitchelson, J. K., Allison, L. K., & Clark, M. A. (2010). Team composition, cognition, and effectiveness: Examining mental model simi- larity and accuracy. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 14, 174- 191. doi:10.1037/a0018444

Rico, R., Sánchez-Manzanares, M., Gil, F., & Gibson, C. (2008). Team implicit coor- dination processes: A team knowledge–based approach. Academy of Management Review, 33, 163-184. doi:10.5465/AMR.2008.27751276

Rogelberg, S. G., & Stanton, J. M. (2007). Introduction: Understanding and dealing with organizational survey nonresponse. Organizational Research Methods, 10, 195-209. doi:10.1177/1094428106294693

Ross, L., Greene, D., & House, P. (1977). The “false consensus effect”: An egocen- tric bias in social perception and attribution processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 279-301. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(77)90049-X

Rupp, D. E., Thornton, M. A., Rogelberg, S. G., Olien, J. L., & Berka, G. (2014). The characteristics of quality scholarly submissions: Considerations of author

Guenter et al. 45

team composition and decision making. Journal of Management, 40, 1501-1510. doi:10.1177/0149206314547387

Rynes, S. L. (2007). Time flies when you’re having fun: AMJ, 2005–2007. Academy of Management Journal, 50, 1273-1276. doi:10.5465/AMJ.2007.28165835

Salas, E., Dickinson, T. L., Converse, S. A., & Tannenbaum, S. I. (1992). Toward an understanding of team performance and training. In R. W. Swezey & E. Salas (Eds.), Teams: Their training and performance (pp. 3-29). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Salazar, M. R., Lant, T. K., Fiore, S. M., & Salas, E. (2012). Facilitating innovation in diverse science teams through integrative capacity. Small Group Research, 43, 527-558. doi:10.1177/1046496412453622

Sanna, L. J., & Schwarz, N. (2003). Debiasing the hindsight bias: The role of acces- sibility experiences and (mis) attributions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 287-295. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00528-0

Schilpzand, M. C., Herold, D. M., & Shalley, C. E. (2011). Members’ openness to experience and teams’ creative performance. Small Group Research, 42, 55-76. doi:10.1177/1046496410377509

Schippers, M. C., Den Hartog, D. N., & Koopman, P. L. (2007). Reflexivity in teams: A measure and correlates. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 56, 189-211. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.2006.00250.x

Simsek, Z., Heavey, C., & Jansen, J. J. (2013). Journal impact as a diffusion process: A conceptualization and the case of the Journal of Management Studies. Journal of Management Studies, 50, 1374-1407. doi:10.1111/joms.12007

Skilton, P. F., & Dooley, K. J. (2010). The effects of repeat collaboration on cre- ative abrasion. Academy of Management Review, 35, 118-134. doi:10.5465/ amr.2010.45577886

Solansky, S. T. (2008). Leadership style and team processes in self-managed teams. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies, 14, 332-341. doi:10.1177/1548051808315549

Spitzmuller, M., & Ilies, R. (2010). Do they [all] see my true self? Leader’s rela- tional authenticity and followers’ assessments of transformational leader- ship. European Journal of Work & Organizational Psychology, 19, 304-332. doi:10.1080/13594320902754040

Stewart, G. L. (2006). A meta-analytic review of relationships between team design features and team performance. Journal of Management, 32, 29-55. doi:10.1177/0149206305277792

Stewart, G. L., & Barrick, M. R. (2000). Team structure and performance: Assessing the mediating role of intrateam process and the moderating role of task type. Academy of Management Journal, 43, 135-148. doi:10.2307/1556372

Tsai, W. C., Chi, N. W., Grandey, A. A., & Fung, S. C. (2012). Positive group affective tone and team creativity: Negative group affective tone and team trust as bound- ary conditions. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 33, 638-656. doi:10.1002/ job.775

Van Mierlo, H., Vermunt, J. K., & Rutte, C. G. (2009). Composing group-level con- structs from individual-level survey data. Organizational Research Methods, 12, 368-392. doi:10.1177/1094428107309322

46 Small Group Research 00(0)

Walumbwa, F. O., Avolio, B. J., Gardner, W. L., Wernsing, T. S., & Peterson, S. J. (2008). Authentic leadership: Development and validation of a theory-based measure. Journal of Management, 34, 89-126. doi:10.1177/0149206307308913

Walumbwa, F. O., Luthans, F., Avey, J. B., & Oke, A. (2011). Authentically leading groups: The mediating role of collective psychological capital and trust. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 32, 4-24. (Retraction published 2014, Journal of Organizational Behavior, 2035, p. 2746)

Wang, D., Waldman, D. A., & Zhang, Z. (2014). A meta-analysis of shared lead- ership and team effectiveness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99, 181-198. doi:10.1037/a0034531

Wen, Z., & Fan, X. (2015). Monotonicity of effect sizes: Questioning kappa- squared as mediation effect size measure. Psychological Methods, 20, 193-203. doi:10.1037/met0000029

Wo, D., Ambrose, M., & Schminke, M. (2015). What drives trickle-down effects? A test of multiple mediation processes. Academy of Management Journal, 58, 1848-1868. doi:10.5465/amj.2013.0670

Woolley, A. W., Gerbasi, M. E., Chabris, C. F., Kosslyn, S. M., & Hackman, J. R. (2008). Bringing in the experts: How team composition and collaborative planning jointly shape analytic effectiveness. Small Group Research, 39, 352- 371. doi:10.1177/1046496408317792

Wuchty, S., Jones, B. F., & Uzzi, B. (2007). The increasing dominance of teams in pro- duction of knowledge. Science, 316, 1036-1039. doi:10.1126/science.1136099

Yammarino, F. J., Dionne, S. D., Schriesheim, C. A., & Dansereau, F. (2008). Authentic leadership and positive organizational behavior: A meso, multi-level perspective. The Leadership Quarterly, 19, 693-707. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2008.09.004

Zaccaro, S. J., Rittman, A. L., & Marks, M. A. (2001). Team leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 12, 451-483. doi: 10.1016/S1048-9843(01)00093-5

Author Biographies

Hannes Guenter (PhD, ETH Zurich) is an associate professor of organizational behavior at Maastricht University School of Business and Economics, the Netherlands. His research interests include team effectiveness, team leadership, and voice behavior.

William L. Gardner is the Jerry S. Rawls Chair in Leadership and the Director of the Institute for Leadership Research at Texas Tech University, USA. He serves as Editor-in-Chief for Group & Organization Management and an associate editor for The Leadership Quarterly. His publications focus on leadership, teams, and influence processes within organizations.

Kelly Davis McCauley is an assistant professor of management at West Texas A&M University, USA. Her research focuses on leadership, emotions, and sociological and psychological theories of the self. She has co-authored publications in The Leadership Quarterly, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, and Human Resource Management Review.

Guenter et al. 47

Brandon Randolph-Seng (associate professor of management, Texas A&M University–Commerce, USA) focuses on the social cognitive factors in organizations and entrepreneurship. Among others, his research has appeared in the Academy of Management Review, Behavior & Brain Sciences, and The Leadership Quarterly. He is also an Associate Editor for Management Decision.

Veena P. Prabhu is an associate professor of management at the California State University, Los Angeles, USA. Her research areas currently include social entrepre- neurship, international HRM, and higher education. She has published in the areas of creativity, entrepreneurship, and management, and has presented her work at several national and international conferences.


Comments are closed.